Content » Vol 95, Issue 4

Investigative Report

Body Attention, Ignorance and Awareness Scale: Assessing Relevant Concepts for Physical and Psychological Functioning in Psoriasis

Sylvia van Beugen1,2, Alicja Ograczyk3, Maaike Ferwerda1,2, Jurgen V. Smit4, Manon E. J. Zeeuwen-Franssen5, Elisabeth B. M. Kroft6, Elke M. G. J. de Jong7, Anna Zalewska-Janowska3, A. Rogier T. Donders8, Peter C. M. van de Kerkhof7, Henriët van Middendorp1,2 and Andrea W. M. Evers1,2

1Health, Medical and Neuropsychology Unit, Leiden University, Leiden, 2Department of Medical Psychology, Radboud university medical center, Nijmegen, The Netherlands, 3Psychodermatology Department, Medical University of Lodz, Lodz, Poland, Dermatology Departments, 4Rijnstate Hospital, Velp, 5Canisius-Wilhelmina Ziekenhuis, Nijmegen, and 6Ziekenhuisgroep Twente, Almelo, Departments of 7Dermatology, and 8Health Evidence of Radboud university medical center, Nijmegen, The Netherlands

A certain level of attention to bodily signals may be adaptive in the management of chronic skin conditions, as a lack of attention may lead to inadequate self-care behaviour and, consequently, may affect functioning and treatment outcomes. The purpose of this study was to develop a body awareness questionnaire and to investigate its psychometric properties and physical and psychological correlates in a cross-sectional study in patients with psoriasis (n = 475). The 16-item Body Attention, Ignorance and Awareness Scale demonstrated a 3-factor structure that could be interpreted as body ignorance, body attention, and body awareness (Cronbach’s α of 0.73, 0.74, and 0.68, respectively). Higher body ignorance was significantly related to more physical symptoms and worse psychological functioning. Body attention and body awareness showed small significant correlations with coping and personality. Given the negative influence of impaired psychological functioning on treatment outcomes, it may be clinically important to screen for theses constructs of body awareness in chronic skin conditions. Key words: psoriasis; body awareness; patient-reported outcome; chronic skin conditions; compliance and adherence; health psychology.

Accepted Sep 30, 2014; Epub ahead of print Oct 1, 2014

Acta Derm Venereol 2014; XX: XX–XX.

Sylvia van Beugen, Leiden University, Institute of Psychology, Health, Medical and Neuropsychology Unit, PO Box 9555, NL-2300 RB Leiden, The Netherlands. E-mail: S.van.beugen@fsw.leidenuniv.nl

Having a chronic skin condition can have negative consequences for physical as well as psychological wellbeing (1–4). One of the major problems that patients have to cope with is itch, which accompanies the majority of dermatological diseases (5–10). The prevalence of itch ranges from 50% up to 92% in patients with chronic skin conditions (6–10). Physical symptoms such as itch are known to significantly affect patients’ quality of life (5–7) and are associated with emotional distress (5, 7, 9, 10). In order to manage their chronic skin condition, patients are often required to take an active role in their treatment; the majority of patients need to use topical treatments on a regular basis to prevent deterioration. As better treatment adherence generally results in greater improvement, adequate self-care behaviour is very relevant in these patients (11). In order to respond to symptoms of the skin condition with appropriate self-care behaviour, patients should be aware of the bodily signals that indicate that self-care is required. Thus, the way in which people pay attention to and interpret bodily signals, which is an important aspects of body awareness, may be relevant, especially in patients with chronic skin conditions.

Body awareness can be defined as an attentional focus on, and awareness of, body sensations (12). In previous research, heightened body awareness has been viewed mainly as a mechanism that may be detrimental to health and wellbeing (13, 14) as it may lead to excessive worrying about physical symptoms and somatizing thoughts. These findings originated partly from findings regarding health anxiety (excessive concern about one’s health; 15) and hyper-vigilance (heightened attention; e.g. 16) to bodily symptoms, both of which are associated with an increased attention to bodily signals, high levels of symptom reporting, and negative health outcomes (e.g. 17). Recently, however, an alternative view has emerged that perceives body awareness as an adaptive process that may be useful in the management of chronic conditions, as long as body awareness involves mindful attentiveness to the present moment and not excessive worrying about symptoms (12, 13, 18, 19).

Even though they are different constructs, both alexi­thymia, which is characterized by the inability to identify and verbalize emotions (20), and body awareness show similarities, in the sense that they share a focus on internal awareness processes. Previous research has found moderately negative associations between alexithymia and body awareness (21–23), suggesting that a lack of ability to identify and verbalize emotions may be related to a lack of awareness of physical sensations.

In samples of dermatological patients, a certain level of body awareness may lead to patients paying adequate attention to bodily symptoms such as dry skin and, resultantly, to taking appropriate action, such as using moisturizing ointments. Subsequently, the lack of body awareness, especially, could be detrimental for patients with a chronic skin condition. For instance, patients who do not pick up subtle bodily signals, such as dry skin or itch, may not take care of their skin accordingly and thus are less able to control their skin condition through adequate skin care. Finally, there are some indirect indications that body awareness interventions may have positive effects on wellbeing for chronic skin conditions. For instance mindful awareness, which includes elements of body awareness, may be of relevance for chronic skin conditions; a brief attention intervention focusing on mindful meditation has shown to accelerate the rate of clearance in psoriasis patients undergoing photo(chemo)therapy (24, 25). Although these results are based on a small study in need of replication, they indicate that (mindful) aspects of body awareness may play a role in alleviating physical symptoms in chronic skin conditions such as psoriasis.

A systematic review examined existing self-report measures for body awareness and concluded that they often lacked adequate psychometric properties and were often unidimensional, leaving out key aspects of body awareness (12). The authors of this review recently constructed a new, relatively elaborate measure of body awareness consisting of 8 subscales as an alternative (18, 26). However, this measure does not include items regarding a specific lack of attention to, or ignorance of, bodily signals, which is hypothesized to be important in dermatological patient samples. The current study aims to: (i) assess a conceptualization of body awareness including ignorance of bodily signals in psoriasis patients by means of a new brief scale suitable for dermatological patient groups; and (ii) examine its psychometric properties and correlations with measures of physical and psychological functioning, personality characteristics, coping, and illness cognitions.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Participants and procedure

In order to prevent selection bias, patients were recruited from both academic (Radboud university medical center, Nijmegen, The Netherlands) and non-academic (Canisius-Wilhelmina Hospital, Nijmegen; Rijnstate Hospital, Velp; Ziekenhuis Groep Twente, Almelo, The Netherlands) hospitals, as well as from the Dutch Psoriasis Association. Inclusion criteria were: a minimum age of 18 years and a dermatologist-confirmed psoriasis diagnosis. Exclusion criteria were: illiteracy, pregnancy, presence of other physical conditions that had a larger perceived impact on the daily life than psoriasis, presence of psychiatric comorbidity (according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders; 27) and/or current psychological treatment. The study protocol was approved by the regional medical ethics committee and written informed consent was obtained from each participant. The current study was part of a larger study examining the effectiveness of E-health cognitive behavioural therapy for patients with psoriasis, and made use of questionnaires that were assessed as part of the screening procedure. All participants who completed questionnaires in the period March 2010–May 2013 were included. The resulting sample size of 475 can be considered (highly) sufficient according to often-cited sample size guidelines for factor analysis (28). Sociodemographic characteristics of participants are shown in Table I. Participants’ mean ± SD age was 52 ± 13 years and gender was approximately evenly distributed (56% male). Disease severity was mild to moderate (29). Approximately half of our sample (46%) reported medical conditions in addition to psoriasis, of whom 32% indicated more than one other medical condition. Most commonly reported conditions were: rheumatic disease (100 of 220 patients; 45%), high blood pressure (n = 27; 12%), heart disease (n = 28; 13%), lung disease (n = 26; 12%), diabetes (n = 26; 12%), chronic back/muscle/tendon pain (n = 21; 9.5%), skin disease besides psoriasis (n = 20; 9%), and thyroid disease (n = 14; 6%).

Table I. Sociodemographic characteristics of the study population (n = 475)

Characteristics

Mean ± SD (range)
or n (%)

Sex

Male

266 (56)

Female

209 (44)

Age, years

52.08 ± 13.06 (18–84)

Marital status

Single

56 (12)

Married/living together

380 (80)

Divorced

22 (5)

Widowed

17 (4)

Educational level

Primary education

72 (15)

Secondary education

226 (48)

Tertiary education

175 (37)

Missing

2 (0.4)

Disease severity (SAPASI)

5.12 ± 4.03 (0–33)

Disease duration, years

16.00 ± 14.95 (0–64)

SAPASI: Self-Administered Psoriasis Area and Severity Index; SD: standard deviation.

Item generation and scale construction

Based on a review of the literature, an item pool was generated with (adjusted) items of the following questionnaires: Bermond-Vorst Alexithymia Questionnaire (BVAQ; 20), Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS-20; 30), Body Awareness Questionnaire (BAQ; 31), Kentucky Inventory of Mindfulness Skills (KIMS; 32), Mindful Attention Awareness Scale (MAAS; 33) and Amplification Questionnaire (APQ; 34). Items were adjusted to suit the revised conceptualization of body attention and ignorance (12). For instance, suitable items from the identifying, analyzing, and verbalizing dimensions of the alexithymia questionnaires were rephrased to reflect (a lack of) body awareness instead of a lack of emotional awareness (e.g. “I don’t know what’s going on inside my body” instead of “I don’t know what’s going on inside me” (TAS-20)). In 4 cases, original items were used. Initial item selection was conducted by 3 medical psychologists with a research and/or clinical focus on psychodermatology. The resulting item pool was sent to 5 other clinicians and researchers involved in the field of psychodermatology and to 3 patients involved in patient participation for dermatological research, with the instruction to evaluate the items on relevance, clarity and readability. This process led to a 23-item scale comprising 14 indicative (positively formulated) and 9 contra-indicative (negatively formulated) items (Table II). Respondents were asked to indicate on a 4-point Likert scale to what extent they agree with each statement (1 = not at all, 2 = somewhat, 3 = to a large extent, 4 = completely).

Table II. Rotated factor loadings of the 3-factor solution for the Body Attention, Ignorance and Awareness Scale (BAIAS)

Items

Body
ignorance

Body
attention

Body awareness

Factor I

1. I do not know what is happening inside my bodya

0.68

–0.05

0.00

2. When I am not feeling well physically, I do not know the reasonb

0.64

0.17

0.10

3. I have physical sensations that I can’t quite identifya

0.63

–0.22

0.02

4. I do not know what is going on in my bodyb

0.62

0.16

–0.16

5. I am often confused about what I observe in my bodya

0.62

–0.11

–0.14

6. I find it difficult to describe my physical sensationsb

0.59

0.23

0.09

7. I tend not to notice feelings of physical tension or discomfort until they really grab my attentionc

0.55

0.19

0.17

Factor II

8. I notice changes in my body, such as whether my breathing slows down or speeds upd

–0.04

0.72

0.12

9. I notice when my physical sensations are beginning to changed

0.17

0.69

0.17

10. I pay attention to whether my muscles are tensed or relaxedd

0.03

0.67

0.19

11. I know in advance when I am getting the flue

0.02

0.66

0.10

12. I know I am running a fever without taking my temperaturee

0.08

0.65

0.09

Factor III

13. I think that you should pay attention to your bodyb

0.01

0.05

0.76

14. When my body feels tense, I want to know where this feeling comes fromb

–0.11

0.08

0.68

15. In general I pay attention to my physical sensationsd

0.21

0.31

0.68

16. It is important that you are aware of your bodya

0.01

0.28

0.64

% of variance explained

23.0

16.0

8.4

Item adapted from: aTAS-20 (Toronto Alexithymia Scale), bBVAQ (Bermond-Vorst Alexithymia Questionnaire), cMAAS (Mindful Attention Awareness Scale), dKIMS-obs (Kentucky Inventory of Mindfulness Skills – observe subscale), eBAQ (Body Awareness Questionnaire). Factor loadings ≥ 0.55 printed in bold.

Measures of physical and psychological functioning

The following commonly-used and well-validated self-report questionnaires were used to examine correlations of body awareness with physical and psychological functioning and provide first indications of construct validity:

Physical and psychological wellbeing. The Impact of Chronic Skin Disease on Daily Life (ISDL; 35) was used to measure physical functioning (itch, pain, and scratching behaviour). Symptoms of itch were assessed by means of a 4-item subscale measuring the intensity and duration of itch during the past 4 weeks (Cronbach’s α in current study = 0.92). The answers are indicated on a 4-point Likert scale (ranging from “not at all” to “completely”) and higher scores indicate higher levels of itch. Pain was assessed on a 10-point VAS scale (0 = no pain, 10 = worst pain ever experienced). Scratching behaviour was assessed with 2 subscales: a 3-item conscious scratching subscale that assesses the frequency and duration of scratching behaviour (Cronbach’s α = 0.79) and a 3-item automatic scratching subscale that evaluates scratching behaviour to non-itching stimuli and scratching in the absence of itch or without being aware of it (Cronbach’s α = 0.71). In both scales, higher scores reflect more scratching behaviour.

The 8-item fatigue subscale of the Checklist Individual Strength (CIS; 36) was used to assess fatigue. The subscale consists of 8 items that are responded to on a 7-point Likert scale (ranging from “yes, that is true” to “no, that is not true”), with higher scores reflecting more fatigue (Cronbach’s α = 0.94).

The Self-Administered Psoriasis Area and Severity Index (SAPASI; 37) was used to assess disease severity. On anterior and posterior silhouettes, patients mark the areas that are currently affected by psoriasis. Below the silhouettes, patients can note the redness, thickness, and scaliness of their psoriasis on 3 analogue scales. Scores on the SAPASI can range from 0 (complete remission) to 72 (most severe psoriasis). The SAPASI has shown good reliability and validity (37, 38).

The Dermatology Life Quality Index (DLQI; 39) is a dermatology-specific instrument used to measure quality of life. The questionnaire assesses the impact of chronic skin conditions on several physical, psychological, and social aspects of daily life. It consists of 10 items that are responded to on a 4-point scale (ranging from “very much” to “not at all”). Higher scores reflect poorer quality of life (Cronbach’s α = 0.85).

The Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS; 40) was used to assess anxiety (7 items; Cronbach’s α = 0.82) and depression (7 items; Cronbach’s α = 0.80). The questions refer to the mood in the last week and they are responded to on a 4-point scale. Higher scores reflect higher levels of anxiety and depression.

Coping. Two subscales of the Utrecht Coping List (UCL; 41) were used to assess active coping (problem-focused coping, 7 items, Cronbach’s α = 0.83) and passive coping (avoidance strategies, 8 items, Cronbach’s α = 0.71) related to handling everyday stressful events. Respondents answer on a 4-point Likert scale (from “seldom or almost never” to “very often/frequently”). Higher scores reflect a stronger tendency to use that specific coping strategy.

Illness cognitions. The Illness Cognition Questionnaire (ICQ; 42) was used to measure illness cognitions related to chronic diseases: helplessness (concentration on aversive aspects of the disease), acceptance (positive adaptation to chronic illness with emphasis on decreasing its negative aspects), and perceived benefits (assigning positive meaning to illness). Each subscale consists of 6 statements answered on a 4-point Likert scale (from “not at all” to “completely”), with higher scores indicating higher levels of the illness cognition. The ICQ was found to be highly reliable in the current sample (Cronbach’s α = 0.88 for each scale).

Personality traits. The Eysenck Personality Questionnaire (EPQ; 43) was used to evaluate the personality traits neuroticism (Cronbach’s α = 0.92) and extraversion (Cronbach’s α = 0.87). It consists of 41 items with a dichotomous response format (yes/no), with higher scores reflecting higher levels of the personality trait.

Statistical analysis

Exploratory factor analysis (EFA) with Varimax rotation and Kaiser normalization was performed to assess the underlying structure of the new questionnaire (44). Selection of the number of factors for the most optimal solution was based on conjunctive criteria requiring the eigenvalue of a factor being at least 1.0 and a clear bend in Cattell’s scree test (45). To assess the reliability of the new questionnaire, Cronbach’s α were computed for the 3 scales. To examine associations between body awareness and measures of physical and psychological functioning and sociodemographic characteristics, Pearson correlation coefficients were computed for measures of physical and psychological wellbeing, personality characteristics, coping, illness cognitions, and sociodemographic characteristics. Statistical significance was accepted at p < 0.05 in two-tailed tests. All analyses were performed using Statistical Packages for the Social Sciences version 20.0 (46).

RESULTS

The newly developed 23-item questionnaire was sent to 682 psoriasis patients, 475 of whom returned the questionnaire (70% response rate). Missing values on this questionnaire were present in 1% of data and were randomly distributed. Missing values were present in 1.6% of data in the entire dataset of included questionnaires (excluding the DLQI, as this scale was not distributed to the entire sample). Due to their low percentage and random distribution, the impact of these missing values is considered to be negligible. No cases with extreme values on one or more variables were observed; all values were within 3 standard deviations of the mean and all variables were normally distributed (skewness and kurtosis < 1.1).

Exploratory factor analysis of the Body Attention, Ignorance and Awareness Scale

The 23 items of the original questionnaire were reduced to 16, based on examination of item loadings and cross-loadings (i.e. primary factor loadings >0.50 and at least 0.20 difference in cross-loadings; 6 items removed based on these criteria), meaningfulness and usefulness for the corresponding factor (1 additional item removed). Based on these 16 items, 3 factors were extracted, explaining a total of 47.4% of the variance. The resulting questionnaire was termed the Body Attention, Ignorance and Awareness Scale (BAIAS). The first component, “Body Ignorance” (not recognizing and/or ignoring bodily signals), explained 23.0% of the variance, the second component, “Body Attention” (being aware of and paying mindful attention to bodily signals), added 16.0% and the third component, “Body Awareness” (self-perceived importance of and general attitude towards body awareness), added 8.4%.

Body Attention, Ignorance and Awareness scale: means and associations with sociodemographic variables

Based on the above-mentioned components, mean scores on the 3 scales were calculated by adding the scores of the items and dividing them by the total number of items of that scale. If more than one-third of the items on 1 particular scale were missing, the mean score of the scale was not calculated. Mean scores on BAIAS scales for the total sample, and stratified by age, gender, educational level, and marital status are shown in Table III. Women scored slightly higher than men on Body Awareness (t(469) = –2.61, p < 0.01), no gender differences were found for the other 2 scales. Patients with a higher educational level scored lower on Body Ignorance (F(2,452) = 13.98, p < 0.001) and higher on Body Attention (F(2,452) = 8.78, p < 0.001) and Body Awareness (F(2,453) = 3.78, p = 0.02). No differences according to age group or marital status were found for any of the 3 scales, nor were the subscales of the BAIAS significantly correlated with age (continuous scores; p-values ≥ 0.36).

Table III. Mean scores on the Body Attention, Ignorance and Awareness Scale (BAIAS), stratified by age, gender, education, and marital status

Body ignorance
Mean ± SD

Body attention
Mean ± SD

Body awareness
Mean ± SD

Total sample

13.59 ± 3.51 (n = 470)

12.32 ± 3.31 (n = 470)

11.61 ± 2.26 (n = 471)

Stratified by age

18–44

13.40 ± 3.45 (n = 127)

12.02 ± 3.35 (n = 127)

11.67 ± 2.17 (n = 126)

45–64

13.49 ± 3.61 (n = 261)

12.52 ± 3.31 (n = 260)

11.54 ± 2.30 (n = 262)

65–84

14.18 ± 3.27 (n = 82)

12.15 ± 3.23 (n = 83)

11.74 ± 2.28 (n = 83)

Stratified by gender

Male

13.76 ± 3.67 (n = 262)

12.35 ± 3.28 (n = 261)

11.37 ± 2.24** (n = 263)

Female

13.37 ± 3.30 (n = 208)

12.28 ± 3.35 (n = 209)

11.91 ± 2.25** (n = 208)

Stratified by education

Primary

15.00 ± 2.93*** (n = 71)

10.85 ± 3.42*** (n = 72)

10.93 ± 2.43* (n = 72)

Secondary

13.82 ± 3.32*** (n = 219)

12.61 ± 3.26*** (n = 218)

11.69 ± 2.29* (n = 219)

Tertiary

12.59 ± 3.61*** (n = 165)

12.58 ± 3.16*** (n = 165)

11.75 ± 2.13* (n = 165)

Stratified by marital status

Married/long-term relationship

13.51 ± 3.53 (n = 398)

12.24 ± 3.21 (n = 399)

11.61 ± 2.22 (n = 400)

Single

14.07 ± 3.42 (n = 71)

12.67 ± 3.79 (n = 70)

11.61 ± 2.52 (n = 70)

*p < 0.05; **p <0.01; ***p <0.001.

SD: standard deviation.

Reliability and intercorre­lations of the Body Atten­tion, Ignorance and Aware­ness Scale

The internal consistency was satisfactory for the Body Ignorance (α = 0.73), Body Attention (α = 0.74) and Body Awareness (α = 0.68) scales of the BAIAS. The subscale Body Attention showed a moderate correlation with the subscale Body Awareness (r = 0.43, p < 0.001), and the Body Ignorance and Body Attention subscales were slightly, but significantly, negatively correlated (r = –0.16, p < 0.001). No significant correlation was found between the Body Awareness and Body Ignorance subscales (r = –0.08, p = 0.09).

As reported in Table IV, higher levels of Body Ignorance were associated with higher levels of itch, pain, and fatigue, more scratching, a decreased quality of life, higher levels of anxious and depressive symptoms, more avoidant coping, less active coping, higher levels of neuroticism and helplessness, and lower levels of extraversion and acceptance. Correlations between the Body Attention subscale and measures of physical and psychological wellbeing were mostly non-significant, with the exception of small correlations of higher Body Attention with higher levels of active coping, acceptance, and extraversion. Similarly, small correlations were found for higher Body Awareness, with higher levels of active coping and higher levels of anxiety, neuroticism and extraversion.

Table IV. Correlations between the Body Attention, Ignorance and Awareness Scale (BAIAS) and measures of physical and psychological functioning

Measure

Body
ignorance

Body
attention

Body awareness

r

n

r

n

r

n

Physical and psychological wellbeing

Itch

0.20***

466

0.00

465

0.06

466

Pain

0.24***

461

–0.05

461

0.03

462

Fatigue

0.32***

468

–0.04

468

0.01

469

Conscious scratching

0.18***

451

–0.05

451

0.03

452

Automatic scratching

0.12*

451

–0.05

451

0.06

451

Quality of life

0.25***

362a

0.02

362a

0.03

362a

Disease severity

0.06

448

0.00

449

0.05

449

Depression

0.33***

467

–0.04

467

–0.07

469

Anxiety

0.34***

468

0.01

468

0.11*

469

Coping

Active

–0.24***

465

0.27***

465

0.18***

466

Passive

0.29***

466

–0.07

466

–0.06

467

Illness cognitions

Helplessness

0.30***

467

0.04

466

0.02

467

Acceptance

–0.24***

467

0.10*

466

0.02

467

Perceived benefits

0.10*

464

0.07

463

0.07

464

Personality traits

Neuroticism

0.33***

469

–0.05

469

0.12**

470

Extraversion

–0.23***

468

0.12**

468

0.14**

469

aQuality of life was not assessed in the whole study sample.

*p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001.

DISCUSSION

The aims of the current study were to develop a measure to assess body awareness and to examine its physical and psychological correlates in patients with psoriasis. The results showed that body awareness can be measured adequately in patients with psoriasis by means of the BAIAS. In addition, body ignorance and, to a lesser extent, body awareness and attention were related to physical and psychological functioning. In particular, patients who do not recognize and/or ignore their bodily signals experience lower levels of physical and psychological wellbeing. The BAIAS showed a 3-factor structure, with 3 reliable scales – body ignorance (not recognizing and/or ignoring bodily signals), body attention (being aware of and paying mindful attention to specific internal bodily signals) and body awareness (self-perceived importance of and general attitude towards body awareness). In line with our hypothesis, the aspect of the BAIAS reflecting a low level of body awareness demonstrated associations with poorer physical and psychological wellbeing. Higher levels of body attention and body awareness were not related to physical and psychological wellbeing, but slightly to aspects of coping and personality that are generally perceived as adaptive (active coping and extraversion). We explicitly added items in the BAIAS that assess the failure to recognize bodily signals, which was shown to be only slighly negatively correlated with body attention. Results suggest that body awareness is not a unidimensional construct, but rather consists of multiple aspects that may be independently associated with physical and psychological functioning. The concepts of body awareness and body ignorance seem to have inherently different meanings. While body awareness refers to the general tendency to be aware of and pay attention to bodily signals, body ignorance is related more to not recognizing or to actively ignoring bodily signals. Several mechanisms can be suggested to play a role in both concepts. For example, parallels can be drawn between body ignorance and the emotion regulation strategies of avoidance and suppression, which were found to be related to psychopathology in meta-analysis with moderate to large effect sizes (47). Body ignorance may also be related to the maladaptive emotion regulation construct of “experimental avoidance”; the suppression or avoidance of a broader range of psychological experiences, including bodily signals, but also, for instance, cognitions, emotions, memories and sensations (48, 49). Body ignorance might be related to problems with adherence; bodily signals often have a certain threat value due to signals of disease, and are therefore potentially avoided or ignored in patients who score high on body ignorance. In contrast, the construct body attention is hypothesized and operationalized to be more related to mindfulness and perhaps a broad awareness of, or sensitivity to, signals in general. When this sensitivity is accompanied by excessive worrying about bodily signals, this process may become maladaptive, as in health anxiety (15). Future research should examine these hypotheses and constructs further. The body ignorance subscale was found to be associated with more physical symptoms of itch, pain, and fatigue, as well as more scratching behaviour. Because treatment of chronic skin conditions relies heavily on adequate self-care behaviour and treatment compliance (11), being aware of bodily sensations and taking appropriate action may be especially important in this population. In addition, not being conscious of bodily sensations such as itch could also lead to unaware, automatic ways of dealing with these sensations, for instance through more frequent automatic scratching behaviour. Scratching may cause further damage to the skin and lead to more itch, a process called the itch-scratch cycle.

Body ignorance was also found to be correlated with psychological wellbeing; patients not recognizing or ignoring bodily signals were found to be more anxious and depressed. In addition, they scored higher on generally maladaptive personality traits and coping styles. These findings may suggest that patients who do not recognize or pay attention to their bodily signals may be at risk for psychological problems. This may be relevant not only for patient wellbeing, but also for dermatological treatment outcomes, as it is known that psychological distress plays a significant role in the exacerbation of psoriasis (50). An alternative explanation could be that patients with high levels of psychological distress may be less aware of their bodily signals as they are focused on exaggerated worries of, for example, health anxiety. Further research is needed on the association between body awareness in its current conceptualization and these distress-related constructs of health anxiety and hyper-vigilance, which are known to be associated with both increased symptom reporting and increased attention to bodily signals (e.g. 17).

The BAIAS was also found to be correlated with sociodemographic variables; women scored slightly higher than men on the body awareness subscale, and a higher educational level was slightly associated with lower levels of body ignorance and higher levels of body attention. These results correspond with the notion that women (51) and individuals with a higher educational level (52) have also been found to score lower on the related emotion regulation construct alexithymia.

The findings of this study should be considered in light of its limitations, which may be addressed in future research. Firstly, as this is the first study on this newly developed questionnaire, the BAIAS should be further validated based on these results. Also, this study was conducted in a sample of psoriasis patients with relatively mild to moderate disease severity from various hospitals, which raises the issue of representativeness and possible floor effects in the development of the questionnaire. However, all 3 body awareness subscales showed a normal distribution and few participants scored the highest or lowest possible values, indicating that floor effects were not likely to be a problem. Moreover, none of the 3 body awareness scales correlated with disease severity. In addition, disease severity in psoriasis is known to be, on average, mild to moderate in general practice (e.g. 6, 53). However, in order to generalize beyond this group, studies need to be conducted that replicate these results in patients with more severe disease activity, other chronic conditions, such as atopic dermatitis, and healthy participants. The developed questionnaire should also be administered to other independent samples to confirm its factor structure and further test its psychometric properties, such as test-retest reliability and sensitivity to change. In addition, it would be informative to perform a qualitative assessment on comprehension of the scale for respondents with diverse educational levels and cultural backgrounds, in order to examine whether the scale is equally comprehensible for all respondents.

Secondly, a substantial part of our sample had a co-morbid medical condition. As research clearly suggests that psoriasis patients are at an increased risk for several other chronic somatic conditions, such as diabetes and cardiovascular conditions (see for an overview: 54), excluding these patients would leave us with a sample that would probably not be very clinically representative. However, this leaves the possibility that differences in body awareness according to medical conditions may have influenced our results. For example, diabetics are taught to be attuned to physiological sensations as they need to detect possible hypoglycaemia in an early stage. Even though in the current study no differences in body awareness between patients with and without these medical conditions were observed, future studies should shed more light on this matter.

Thirdly, while correlations between the body ignorance scale and psychological and physical functioning were consistently in the same direction, the magnitude of the correlations was generally small to moderate. This corresponds with the knowledge that a multitude of factors can contribute to poor physical and psychological wellbeing, with body awareness being one of these factors. Lastly, the cross-sectional nature of this study precludes causal conclusions. While it could be hypothesized that a low awareness of bodily signals leads to poorer self-care behaviour and, therefore, poorer skin status and lower physical and psychological wellbeing, alternative hypotheses cannot be ruled out. Future prospective studies should disentangle the causal pathways between body awareness and wellbeing.

In conclusion, the current study provided new insights into the potential relevance of body ignorance, body attention, and body awareness, which can be reliably measured with the BAIAS. Body ignorance was found to be associated with worse physical and psychological functioning. If it is proven that body ignorance is predictive of worse functioning in longitudinal studies, the BAIAS may be useful in assessment and screening for body ignorance in patients with psoriasis. Subsequently, psychological interventions, such as mindfulness or body awareness training, may be helpful in reducing body ignorance and improving body attention and awareness in an adequate manner (55–59).

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

This study was supported by a research grant (WS682746) from Pfizer Inc. Pfizer was not involved in the study design, data collection, data analysis, manuscript preparation or publication decisions.

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

REFERENCES

1. Evers AWM, Lu Y, Duller P, van der Valk PGM, Kraaimaat FW, van de Kerkhof PCM. Common burden of chronic skin diseases? Contributors to psychological distress in adults with psoriasis and atopic dermatitis. Br J Dermatol 2005; 152: 1275–1281.

2. Fortune DG, Richards HL, Griffiths, CE. Psychologic factors in psoriasis: consequences, mechanisms, and interventions. Dermatol Clin 2005; 23: 681–694.

3. Rabin F, Bhuiyan SI, Islam T, Haque MA, Islam MA. Psychiatric and psychological comorbidities in patients with psoriasis – a review. Mymensingh Med J 2012; 21: 780–786.

4. Rapp SR, Exum ML, Reboussin DM, Feldman SR, Fleischer A, Clark A. The physical, psychological and social impact of psoriasis. J Health Psychol 1997; 2: 525–537.

5. Verhoeven EWM, de Klerk S, Kraaimaat FW, van de Kerkhof PCM, de Jong EMGJ, Evers AWM. Biopsychosocial mechanisms of chronic itch in patients with skin diseases: a review. Acta Derm Venereol 2008; 88: 211–218.

6. Verhoeven EWM, Kraaimaat FW, van de Kerkhof PCM, van Weel C, Duller P, van der Valk PGM, et al. Prevalence of physical symptoms of itch, pain and fatigue in patients with skin diseases in general practice. Br J Dermatol 2007; 156: 1346–1349.

7. Yosipovitch G, Goon A, Wee J, Chan YH, Goh CL. The prevalence and clinical characteristics of pruritus among patients with extensive psoriasis. Br J Dermatol 2000; 143: 969–973.

8. Sampogna F, Gisondi P, Melchi CF, Amerio P, Girolomoni G, Abeni D, et al. Prevalence of symptoms experienced by patients with different clinical types of psoriasis. Br J Dermatol 2004; 151: 594–599.

9. Prignano F, Ricceri F, Pescitelli L, Lotti T. Itch in psoriasis: epidemiology, clinical aspects and treatment options. Clin Cosmet Investig Dermatol 2009; 2: 9–13.

10. Gupta MA, Gupta AK, Kirkby S, Weiner HK, Mace TM, Schork NJ, et al. Pruritus in psoriasis. A prospective study of some psychiatric and dermatologic correlates. Arch Dermatol 1988; 124: 1052–1057.

11. Carroll CL, Feldman SR, Camacho FT, Balkrishnan R. Better medication adherence results in greater improvement in severity of psoriasis. Br J Dermatol 2004; 151: 895–897.

12. Mehling WE, Gopisetty V, Daubenmier J, Price CJ, Hecht FM, Stewart A. Body awareness: construct and self-report measures. PloS One 2009; 4: e5614.

13. Cioffi D. Beyond attentional strategies: cognitive-perceptual model of somatic interpretation. Psychol Bull 1991; 109: 25–41.

14. Abramowitz JS, Schwartz SA, Whiteside SP. A contemporary conceptual model of hypochondriasis. Mayo Clin Proc 2002; 77: 1323–1330.

15. Taylor S, Asmundson, GJG. Treating health anxiety: a cognitive-behavioral approach. New York: Guilford Press, 2004.

16. Crombez G, Van Damme S, Eccleston C. Hypervigilance to pain: an experimental and clinical analysis. Pain 2005; 116: 4–7.

17. Haenen MA, Schmidt AJ, Kroeze S, van den Hout MA. Hypochondriasis and symptom reporting – the effect of attention versus distraction. Psychother Psychosom 1996, 65, 43–48.

18. Mehling WE, Price C, Daubenmier JJ, Acree M, Bartmess E, Stewart A. The Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness (MAIA). PloS One 2012; 7: e48230.

19. Watkins E, Teasdale JD. Adaptive and maladaptive self-focus in depression. J Affect Disord 2004; 82: 1–8.

20. Vorst HCM, Bermond B. Validity and reliability of the Bermond-Vorst Alexithymia Questionnaire. Pers Individ Dif 2001; 30: 413–434.

21. Ernst J, Böker H, Hättenschwiler J, Schüpbach D, Northoff G, Seifritz E, Grimm S, et al. The association of interoceptive awareness and alexithymia with neurotransmitter concentrations in insula and anterior cingulate. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2013; 9: 857–863.

22. Herbert BM, Herbert C, Pollatos O. On the relationship between interoceptive awareness and alexithymia: is interoceptive awareness related to emotional awareness? J Pers 2011; 79: 1149–1175.

23. Mehling WE, Krause N. Are difficulties perceiving and expressing emotions associated with low-back pain?: The relationship between lack of emotional awareness (alexithymia) and 12-month prevalence of low-back pain in 1180 urban public transit operators. J Psychosom Res 2005; 58: 73–81.

24. Benhard JD, Kristeller J, Kabat-Zinn J. Effectiveness of relaxation and visualization techniques as an adjunct to phototherapy and photochemotherapy of psoriasis. J Am Acad Dermatol 1988; 19: 572–573.

25. Kabat-Zinn J, Wheeler E, Light T, Skillings A, Scharf MJ, Cropley TG, et al. Influence of a mindfulness meditation-based stress reduction intervention on rates of skin clearing in patients with moderate to severe psoriasis undergoing phototherapy (UVB) and photochemotherapy (PUVA). Psychosom Med 1998; 60: 625–632.

26. Mehling WE, Daubenmier J, Price CJ, Acree M, Bartmess E, Stewart AL, et al. Self-reported interoceptive awareness in primary care patients with past or current low back pain. J Pain Res 2013; 6: 403–418.

27. American Psychiatric Association: DSM-IV – Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th edn. Washington: American Psychiatric Association, 1994.

28. Comrey AL. A first course in factor analysis. New York: Academic Press. Inc.; 1973.

29. Feldman SR, Fleischer AB, Jr, Reboussin DM, Rapp SR, Exum ML, Clark AR, et al. The self-administered psoriasis area and severity index is valid and reliable. J Investig Dermatol 1996; 106: 183–186.

30. Bagby RM, Taylor GJ, Parker JD. The Twenty-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale-II. Convergent, discriminant, and concurrent validity. J Psychosom Res 1994; 38: 33–40.

31. Shields SA, Mallory ME, Simon A. The Body Awareness Questionnaire: reliability and validity. J Pers Assess 1989; 53: 802–815.

32. Baer RA, Smith GT, Allen KB. Assessment of mindfulness by self-report: the Kentucky inventory of mindfulness skills. Assessment 2004; 11: 191–206.

33. Brown KW, Ryan RM. The benefits of being present: mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. J Pers Soc Psychol 2003; 84: 822–848.

34. Barsky AJ, Goodson JD, Lane RS, Cleary PD. The amplification of somatic symptoms. Psychosom Med 1988; 50: 510–519.

35. Evers AWM, Duller P, van de Kerkhof PCM, van der Valk PGM, de Jong EMGJ, Gerritsen MJP, et al. The Impact of Chronic Skin Disease on Daily Life (ISDL): a generic and dermatology-specific health instrument. Br J Dermatol 2008; 158: 101–108.

36. Vercoulen JHMM, Swanink CMA, Fennis JFM, Galama JMD, van der Meer JWM, Bleijenberg G. Dimensional assessment of chronic fatigue syndrome. J Psychosom Res 1994; 38: 383–392.

37. Fleischer AB, Jr, Feldman SR, Dekle CL. The SAPASI is valid and responsive to psoriasis disease severity changes in a multi-center clinical trial. J Dermatol 1999; 26: 210–215.

38. Fleischer AB, Jr, Feldman SR, Rapp SR, Reboussin DM, Exum ML, Clark AR, et al. Disease severity measures in a population of psoriasis patients: the symptoms of psoriasis correlate with self-administered psoriasis area severity index scores. J Invest Dermatol 1996; 107: 26–29.

39. Finlay AY, Khan GK. Dermatology Life Quality Index (DLQI) – a simple practical measure for routine clinical use. Clin Exp Dermatol 1994; 19: 210–216.

40. Zigmond AS, Snaith RP. The hospital anxiety and depression scale. Acta Psychiatr Scand 1983; 67: 361–370.

41. Schreurs PJG, Willige van de G, Brosschot JF, Tellegen B, Graus GMH. De Utrechtse Coping Lijst: UCL, Handleiding [Manual of the Utrecht Coping List]. Lisse: Swets en Zeitlinger, 1993.

42. Evers AWM, Kraaimaat FW, van Lankveld W, Jongen PJH, Jacobs JWG, Bijlsma JWJ. Beyond unfavorable thinking: the illness cognition questionnaire for chronic diseases. J Consult Clin Psychol 2001; 69: 1026–1036.

43. Eysenck HJ. Manual of the Eysenck Personality Scales (EPS Adult). London: Hodder & Stoughton, 1991.

44. Tabachnick BG, Fidell LS. Using multivariate statistics. 5th edn. Boston: Allyn and Bacon, 2007.

45. Cattell RB. Handbook of multivariate experimental psychology. Chicago: Rand McNally, 1966.

46. IBM Corp. Released 2011. IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 20.0. Armonk, New York: IBM Corp.

47. Aldao A, Nolen-Hoeksema S, Schweizer S. Emotion-regulation strategies across psychopathology: a meta-analytic review. Clin Psychol Rev 2010; 30: 217–237.

48. Hayes SC, Strosahl KD, Wilson KG. Acceptance and commitment therapy: an experiential approach to behavior change. New York: Guilford Press, 1999.

49. Hayes SC, Wilson KG, Gifford EV, Follette VM, Strosahl K. Experiential avoidance and behavioral disorders: a func­tional dimensional approach to diagnosis and treatment. J Consult Clin Psychol 1996; 64: 1152–1168.

50. Hunter HJA, Griffiths CEM, Kleyn CE. Does psychosocial stress play a role in the exacerbation of psoriasis? Br J Dermatol 2013; 169: 965–974.

51. Levant RF, Hall, RJ, Williams, CM, Hasan, NT. Gender differences in alexithymia. Psychol Men Masc 2009; 10: 190–203.

52. Pasini A, Delle Chiaie R, Seripa S, Ciani N. Alexithymia as related to sex, age, and educational level: results of the Toronto Alexithymia Scale in 417 normal subjects. Compr Psychiatry 1992; 33: 42–46.

53. Nevitt GJ, Hutchinson PE. Psoriasis in the community: prevalence, severity and patients’ beliefs and attitudes towards the disease. Br J Dermatol 1996; 135: 533–537.

54. Kimball AB, Gladman D, Gelfand JM, Gordon K, Horn EJ, Korman NJ, et al. National Psoriasis Foundation clinical consensus on psoriasis comorbidities and recommendations for screening. J Am Acad Dermatol 2008; 58, 1031–1042.

55. Morone NE, Greco CM, Weiner DK. Mindfulness meditation for the treatment of chronic low back pain in older adults: a randomized controlled pilot study. Pain 2008; 134: 310–319.

56. Kabat-Zinn J. An outpatient program in behavioral medicine for chronic pain patients based on the practice of mindfulness meditation: theoretical considerations and preliminary results. Gen Hosp Psychiatry 1982; 4: 33–47.

57. Berman RLH, Iris MA, Bode R, Drengenberg C. The effectiveness of an online mind-body intervention for older adults with chronic pain. J Pain 2009; 10: 68–79.

58. Astin JA. Mind-body therapies for the management of pain. Clin J Pain 2004; 20: 27–32

59. Mehling WE, Wrubel J, Daubenmier JJ, Price CJ, Kerr CE, Silow T, et al. Body Awareness: a phenomenological inquiry into the common ground of mind-body therapies. Philos Ethics Humanit Med 2011; 6: 6.