Content » Vol 52, Issue 1

Original report

SEX DIFFERENCES IN THE EFFECTS OF EXERCISE ON COGNITION POST-STROKE: SECONDARY ANALYSIS OF A RANDOMIZED CONTROLLED TRIAL

Shereen Khattab, BSc, MSc1, Janice J. Eng, PT/OT, PhD2, Teresa Liu-Ambrose, PT, PhD2, Julie Richardson, PT, PhD1, Joy MacDermid, PT, PhD3 and Ada Tang, PT, PhD1

From the 1School of Rehabilitation Science, McMaster University, Hamilton, Ontario, 2Department of Physical Therapy, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia and 3Department of Physical Therapy, University of Western, London, Ontario, Canada

Abstract

Objective: To determine whether there are differences in exercise-associated changes in cognitive func-tion between males and females living with stroke.

Design: Secondary analysis of data from a prospective assessor-blinded randomized controlled trial.

Participants: Fifty participants (50–80 years, > 1 year post-stroke, able to walk ≥ 5 m).

Methods: Participants were allocated into a 6-month aerobic exercise programme (14 males, 11 females) or balance and flexibility programme (15 males, 10 females). Working memory (Verbal Digit Span Backwards Test), selective attention and conflict resolution (Stroop Colour-Word Test), and set shifting/cognitive flexibility (Trail-Making Test B) were assessed before and after the programmes.

Results: There was a group × time interaction in females (effect size 0.28, p = 0.03), which was not observed in males (effect size 0.01, p = 0.62). Females demonstrated a Stroop Colour-Word Interference test change of –2.3 s, whereas males demonstrated a change of +5.5 s following aerobic exercise. There were no differences between exercise groups in either sex for any of the other outcomes (working memory and set-shifting/cognitive flexibility).

Conclusion: Females living with stroke may demonstrate a greater response to exercise on selective attention and conflict resolution compared with males with stroke. These findings suggest that there may be sex-specific effects of exercise on cognitive func-tion in individuals with stroke.

Key words: sex; cognition; stroke; rehabilitation; exercise; randomized controlled trial.

Accepted Oct 2, 2019; Epub ahead of print Oct 14, 2019

J Rehabil Med 2020; 52: jrm00002

Correspondence address: Ada Tang, McMaster University, School of Rehabilitation Science, 1400 Main Street West, Institute of Applied Health Sciences 403, Hamilton Ontario L8S 1C7, Canada. E-mail: atang@mcmaster.ca

Lay Abstract

Exercise can improve thinking and memory in people with stroke, but we do not know if men and women improve to the same degree. This study examined whether there were differences in thinking and memory between men and women with stroke after 6 months of aerobic or balance/flexibility exercise. Volunteers were randomly assigned to a 6-month aerobic exercise programme or a balance/flexibility exercise programme. Thinking and memory skills were tested before and after the exercise programme. Women did better then men on a test of attention and problem-solving after exercise. There were no differences between men and women in any other test. These findings show that exercise may be better for improving attention and problem-solving in women living with stroke than in men.

Introduction

Stroke is one of the leading causes of mortality and morbidity worldwide, and the sequelae from stroke have physical, psychosocial and cognitive consequences (1). While individuals may recover from physical disability resulting from stroke, cognitive impairments may limit the extent of independent living and community re-integration (2, 3). Approximately 70% of individuals with stroke experience cognitive impairments (4, 5), associated with functional decline (6, 7), reduced quality of life (6) and increased dependence in activities of daily living (8).

Exercise may benefit cognition in individuals living with stroke, such as executive function and memory (9, 10). Evidence in both single group studies and randomized controlled trials suggest that exercise may improve cognitive function after stroke. In single group studies, 12 weeks of aerobic and resistance training improved working memory, but not attention or executive function in a population of 9 individuals >12 months post-stroke (11). Six months of combined aerobic and resistance training led to improvements in global cognitive function in 41 patients ≥10 weeks post-stroke (12), and a 6-month exercise and recreation intervention improved cognitive flexibility, verbal memory and response inhibition, but not working memory or task-switching, in 11 participants ≥12 months post-stroke (13). In randomized controlled trials, the findings have been less consistent. Quaney and colleagues (14) found that an 8-week upper body cycling training programme was more effective at improving motor learning, processing speed, implicit memory, and motor function, but not executive func-tion, compared with a stretching control intervention in 38 participants ≥ 6 months post-stroke. Liu-Ambrose & Eng (15) demonstrated that a 6-month exercise and recreation programme resulted in improved selective attention and conflict resolution, and working memory in 28 individuals ≥ 12 months post- stroke. In contrast, Tang et al. (16) found no changes in working memory, set-shifting/cognitive flexibility, or selective attention and conflict resolution following 6 months of high- or low-intensity exercise in 47 individuals > 1 year post-stroke. Recently, a systematic review of 14 studies and 736 participants with stroke reported improvements in attention and processing speed, but not in executive function or working memory following physical activity training (17). These inconsistencies in the literature may be due to differences in cognitive domains assessed, outcome measures, exercise protocols used, or participant characteristics (17).

Sex refers to biological features related to physical and physiological aspects, such as hormone function, gene expression, chromosomes, and reproductive/sexual anatomy (18), whereas gender is a socially constructed attribute influencing how individuals behave, interact, and perceive themselves and others. Older females are known to be at higher risk of stroke than males (19), have higher rates of stroke-related mortality (19), present with more co-morbidities (20, 21), and experience more severe disability (20, 21). Sex may also be a potential moderator of the effects of exercise on cognitive function in older adults, given that female sex may positively influence the strength of the relationship between exercise and cognition (22, 23). An early systematic review published in 2003 (18 studies, 197 participants) reported that studies involving a high proportion of healthy older females demonstrated greater improvements in cognition following exercise training compared with studies involving a high proportion of healthy older males (22). In 33 older adults with mild cognitive impairment, executive function improved to a greater degree in females compared with males following 6 months of aerobic training (24). In addition, in 152 older adults with mild cognitive impairment, females demonstrated greater improvements in attention and memory following 12 months of moderate intensity walking (25). Recently, a systematic review of 41 studies involving 5,156 older adults reported that exercise was associated with greater improvements in executive functioning in studies involving a high proportion of females compared with a low proportion of females (26).

To date, there have been no studies examining sex differences in cognition following exercise interventions in individuals with stroke. Therefore, the objective of this study was to determine whether males and females living with stroke demonstrate different responses with respect to changes in working memory, set-shifting/cognitive flexibility and selective attention and conflict resolution following 6 months of aerobic or balance and flexibility exercise. Given that preliminary literature has displayed sex-specific improvements in cognition, favouring females, it was hypothesized that females with stroke would demonstrate greater improvements in executive functioning following aerobic exercise vs. balance and flexibility exercise compared with males.

METHODS

This study was a secondary analysis of data from a prospective assessor-blinded randomized controlled trial (27) that examined the effects of aerobic and balance and flexibility exercise on cardiovascular outcomes in 50 participants living in the community with stroke (Clinical Trial Registration; http://www.clinicaltrials.gov. Unique identifier: NCT01189045). Details of the main study are provided elsewhere (27), but, in brief, individuals were eligible if they were: between the ages of 50 and 80 years, > 1 year post-stroke, and able to walk ≥ 5 m. Exclusion criteria were: stroke from aneurysm, tumour, infection or significant health conditions that would preclude participation in exercise (presence of cardiovascular abnormalities, pacemaker, serious musculoskeletal or other conditions). Eligible participants were randomized into either a 6-month aerobic exercise (AE) programme or balance and flexibility (BF) programme using a 1:1 allocation sequence and permuted block sizes of 2 or 4. The flow of participants throughout the study is shown in Fig. 1.


Fig. 1. Consolidated Standards of Reporting Trials (CONSORT) diagram depicting participant flow through study.

The sample size calculated for the original analysis was determined as n = 24 per group. This was based on a 10% change in aerobic capacity, the primary outcome of the original study, utilizing an alpha level of 0.05, type II error of 90% and a standard deviation (SD) of 3 (27). While the original sample size was not established to examine subgroups of secondary outcomes, this was a preliminary analysis of sex differences in cognitive function following exercise after stroke.

Assessments

Upon study entry, participants were assessed for stroke severity and motor recovery using the National Institutes of Health Stroke Scale (28) and the Chedoke-McMaster Stroke Assessment (29), respectively. Aerobic capacity was assessed using a graded maximal exercise cycle ergometry test (27), and walking ability using 5-m self-selected gait speed and the 6-Minute Walk Test (30). The Montreal Cognitive Assessment (31, 32) and Center for Epidemiologic Studies– Depression Scale (33) were used to evaluate global cognitive function and presence of depressive symptoms, respectively.

Cognitive outcome measures

The Verbal Digit Span Backward Test (34), Trail-Making Test B (35, 36), and Stroop Test (37) were conducted to evaluate the effects of exercise on executive functioning at baseline and at 6 months (post-exercise). These outcome measures were selected based on the difficulties with executive functions (cognitive processes responsible for controlled, goal-directed behaviour) commonly observed after stroke (16).

The Verbal Digit Span Backwards Test (34) assesses working memory by verbally presenting random number sequences of increasing length to the participant, who then relayed the numbers back to the examiner in reverse order. One point was scored for each correct sequence relayed (maximum score 14); higher scores indicate better performance (34). The Verbal Digit Span Backwards Test has moderate concurrent validity with the Executive Function Performance Test in people with stroke (38).

The Trail-Making Test Part B (35, 36) was used to assess set shifting/cognitive flexibility, where the participant traced a line alternating between encircled numbers and letters (i.e. 1-A-2-B-3-C, etc.). The test was timed (in seconds, maximum 300 s), where shorter times indicate better cognitive flexibility (36). Selective attention and conflict resolution (problem-solving) were assessed using the Color-Word Stroop Test (37), where the participant identified the ink colour of words presented in incongruent coloured inks (e.g. the word “blue” printed in red ink). The time (in s) required to complete the test was recorded, where shorter times indicate better performance (37). The Trail-Making Test Part B and Color-Word Stroop test have good test-retest reliability in people with stroke and older adults, respectively (37, 39, 40).

Interventions

Both interventions took place 3×/week at 60 min/session with 3 instructors per 12–13 participants. In the aerobic exercise (AE) programme, exercise was performed at intensities that progressed from 40% to 80% of heart rate reserve. Each AE session involved a 10-min warm-up and cool-down and a 30–40-min aerobic component (walking, cycle ergometry, marching-on-the-spot, repeated sit-to-stand, and step ups onto platform steppers). In the BF programme, exercise was non-aerobic in nature and intensity was maintained below 40% heart rate reserve. Participants in the BF programme progressed through activities involving stretching, postural awareness, balance exercises and weight bearing. In order to minimize contamination between groups, the exercise classes were held at different times of the day and different spaces within a multi-purpose research facility.

Statistical analysis

Descriptive statistics were performed for all variables by sex (mean (SD) or median (IQR) for continuous variables and frequencies for categorical variables). In addition, χ2 or Fisher’s exact tests (if expected counts were less than 5) were performed for all categorical variables and independent t-tests or Mann–Whitney U tests were conducted for all continuous variables, by sex.

To evaluate the effects of exercise on cognition, disaggregated analysis (18) using a 2-way mixed analyses of covariance were conducted for each cognitive test, for each sex, with age (years) and baseline Montreal Cognitive Assessment scores (continuous variable) included as covariates. Older age is an important predictor of cognitive outcome and development of cognitive impairment post-stroke (41), and baseline cognitive status may be a factor influencing the extent of change. There is no non-parametric equivalent of the 2-way mixed analyses of covariance; thus, this method was used regardless if normality was met, due to the robust nature of the test. Data were analysed using Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (Version 23.0, Chicago, IL, USA). A significance level of p < 0.05 was used.

RESULTS

Baseline characteristics for the 50 participants enrolled in the study, separated by sex and group, are shown in Table I. While National Institute of Health Stroke Scale values indicate that participants had a mild severity of stroke, mean aerobic capacity and 6-Minute Walk Test scores were 53.7% (SD 138) (42) and 60% (SD 28.6) (43) of normative values, respectively. Males and females were similar in all characteristics at baseline with the exception of mean aerobic capacity (males 19.5 ml/kg/min (SD  6.4), females 12.7 ml/kg/min (SD 3.8), p < 0.001). There were no other differences between groups, within each sex at baseline. Baseline cognitive outcome scores, separated by sex and group, are shown in Table II.

Three (2 females and one male from the aerobic exercise group) of the 50 participants did not complete the study for reasons unrelated to the intervention and were deemed cases missing completely at random. Thus, analyses were performed from 47 participants who completed the main study. Pre- and post-training data on cognitive outcomes were not obtained for 5 participants due to significant aphasia (One female with missing data for Stroop Test and Trail-Making Test B, one male with missing data for Trail-Making Test B), difficulty understanding test instructions (2 males, one with missing data for Stroop Test and one with missing data for Trail-Making Test B) and colour blindness (1 male missing Stroop Test data). These individuals were excluded from the analysis. In addition, for individuals who were missing post-training cognitive data; baseline scores were carried forward.


Table I. Baseline characteristics for males and females, by intervention group


Table II. Baseline scores for cognitive measures for males and females, by intervention group

Data for results of cognitive outcomes are shown in Table III. After controlling for age and baseline Montreal Cognitive Assessment scores, a group × time interaction effect was observed in females for the Stroop test (F = 5.53, p = 0.03, effect size 0.28) that was not seen in males (F = 0.25, p = 0.62, effect size 0.01) (Table III). Specifically, females demonstrated a Stroop Colour-Word Interference test change of –2.3 s, whereas males demonstrated a change of +5.5, following AE. There were no differences between exercise groups in either sex for any of the other outcomes (working memory and set-shifting/cognitive flexibility) (Table III).


Table III. Changes in cognitive function in males and females, controlling for age and Montreal Cognitive Assessment score

DISCUSSION

Our previous analysis concluded that exercise was not effective in improving cognitive function, whereas this study extends this analysis to indicate that females demonstrated improvements in selective attention and conflict resolution following AE, whereas males did not. Other aspects of cognitive function (working memory and set-shifting/cognitive flexibility) did not demonstrate sex differences in treatment response. The positive benefits of exercise observed in females with stroke is consistent with previous studies that also reported selective improvements in the Stroop test performance among females (44, 45). Impairments in executive function are often the first cognitive domains affected by ageing (46), and are common after stroke (47, 48). Older females without known cognitive impairments perform better on tasks of executive function compared with males (49), suggesting that executive function may be selectively preserved in females and thus more responsive to improvement with exercise training, although further research to confirm this finding is warranted (26). Previous studies in the stroke population have also demonstrated exercise-associated improvements in the Stroop test (13, 15, 24, 50), but our results are the first to report that the beneficial effects may be influenced by sex.

The mechanisms underlying sex differences in the effects of exercise on executive function are not known. Hormonal and neurotrophic factors have been proposed as potential factors associated with cognitive function. Sex steroid hormones oestrogen (females) and testosterone (males) are associated with the preservation of cognitive function (23, 51–53), and emerging evidence in older adults suggests that this association may be more pronounced in females (54–56). For example, studies in older adults have demonstrated that, following menopause in females and andropause in males, remaining oestrogen levels (and to a smaller degree testosterone) were associated with improvements in semantic, episodic and working memory, as well as executive function in females only (54–56).

Brain-derived neurotropic factor (BDNF) is a neurotrophic factor that supports neuronal proliferation, growth, survival and synaptic plasticity and is involved in cellular mechanisms required for learning and memory (57). BDNF appears to be a mediator in the relationship between aerobic training and cognitive function, possibly due to its role in enhancing neurogenesis and reducing inflammation in the brain (58). Aerobic exercise results in the upregulation of BDNF in both individuals with (59) and without (60) neurological disorders, such as stroke, multiple sclerosis, Parkinson’s, and Alzheimer’s disease. While BDNF levels are known to decline with age, this decline is associated with cognitive impairment in older females only (61). Recently, it was reported that circulating levels of BDNF increased in older females following 6 months of aerobic training, whereas it decreased in males (62). Oestrogen has also been associated with greater BDNF expression in the hippocampus and cortex of females (63, 64). In animal models, mice exposed to an enriched environment involving running wheels stimulated greater upregulation of BDNF in several brain regions in female mice compared with male mice (65, 66). Similarly, in humans, greater duration, frequency and amount of total daily walking activity was associated with larger hippocampal volume in females, but not males (67). Taken together, these studies suggest that a unique, sex-specific link may exist between exercise and cognition, which may be mediated by BDNF (26).

In contrast to the improvements observed in selective attention and conflict resolution in females, we did not find improvements in other domains of executive function of working memory and set-shifting/cognitive flexibility in either males or females. This finding is aligned with previous research. For example, in a systematic review involving older adults, exercise was effective in improving visuospatial functioning and episodic memory but there were no sex-dependent effects observed (26). This suggests that males and females may perform similarly in certain cognitive domains. While there is still an absence of clear evidence to suggest that clinicians must consider the influence of sex within clinical decision making, it remains important to acknowledge that sex similarities and differences may exist. Further research is warranted.

A possible reason for these domain-specific effects may be that the duration was not sufficient or multi-modal exercise interventions are needed to elicit improvement in more cognitive domains. Specific exercise parameters to optimize improvement in cognitive function after stroke are currently unknown. The duration of exercise may play a role, as research in older adults has suggested that greater benefit is observed with exercise durations greater than 6 months (22). It is also possible that improvements in other cognitive domains may require more complex interventions, as opposed to aerobic training alone. A 12-week combined intervention involving aerobic and resistance exercise and cognitive training resulted in greater improvement in cognitive flexibility, working memory, and selective attention and conflict resolution compared with physical exercise alone in 179 post-stroke individuals (50). Thus, it appears that interventions that use a combined approach, along with those of longer durations, may be required for larger and more diffuse improvements in cognitive function to be observed (68).

Study limitations

The key limitation of this study is that it was not powered to detect changes in male and female subgroups in cognition post-exercise. However, it is likely that this limitation is minimal due to the small effect sizes; thus a fully powered study would potentially find clinically relevant differences. In addition, examining underlying mechanisms of cognitive improvement post-exercise was beyond the scope of this study. Furthermore, when measuring sex in the primary analysis, participants were provided with response options that did not differentiate sex and gender, or allow for non-binary responses, leaving potential for misclassification.

We acknowledge that gender, in addition to sex, may be another moderator in the relationship between exercise and cognitive function (18). Gender encompasses various sociocultural factors, such as education level, previous occupation, and engagement in exercise which can influence health behaviours, the environment and life course. Women access life-saving treatments less frequently than men, and fewer women participate in stroke rehabilitation, have higher institutional rates post-stroke, and have less social support during recovery in comparison to men (69). These sociocultural factors may influence baseline cognitive levels or the magnitude of change in cognitive function following exercise interventions. While we were not able to conduct gender-based analyses in the current study as information related to socio-cultural factors (level of education, hours of work, responsibility for caring for children etc.) were not collected and a gender index could not be created, future research should consider the role of gender in cognitive changes with exercise after stroke.

Conclusion

This study provides preliminary evidence to suggest that there may be sex differences with respect to exercise-associated changes in cognition specifically executive functioning after stroke. However, based on our findings, until more definitive results are reported, there is no clear rationale to support the consideration of sex in designing interventions post-stroke and it may be that males and females can be treated similarly. Given the prevalence of cognitive impairment post-stroke, further research is warranted to better understand this relationship and ultimately develop optimal sex-specific, evidence-based parameters for exercise training for people living with stroke.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

This study was funded by the Vancouver Foundation/Carl and Elsie Halterman Research Fund and the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (CIHR) (MOP-111183). AT was supported by a personnel award from the Heart and Stroke Foundation, Ontario Provincial Office (CS I 7468), the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (MFE-98550) and the Michael Smith Foundation for Health Research (MSFHR) (ST-PDF-03003(11-1) CLIN), and JJE was supported by the CIHR (MSH-63617) and the MSFHR. TLA is a Canada Research Chair in Physical Activity, Mobility, and Cognitive Neuroscience.

The authors have no conflicts of interest to declare.

REFERENCES
  1. Benjamin EJ, Virani SS, Callaway CW, Chamberlain AM, Chang AR, Cheng S, et al. Heart Disease and Stroke Statistics – 2018 update: a report from the American Heart Association. [Cited 2018 Sep 14] Circulation 2018; 137: e67–e492.
    View article    Google Scholar
  2. Wagle J, Farner L, Flekkøy K, Bruun Wyller T, Sandvik L, Fure B, et al. Early post-stroke cognition in stroke rehabilitation patients predicts functional outcome at 13 months. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2011; 31: 379–387.
    View article    Google Scholar
  3. Gottesman RF, Hillis AE. Predictors and assessment of cognitive dysfunction resulting from ischaemic stroke. Lancet Neurol 2010; 9: 895–905.
    View article    Google Scholar
  4. Nys GMS, Zandvoort MJE van, Kort PLM de, Jansen BPW, Haan EHF de, Kappelle LJ. Cognitive disorders in acute stroke: prevalence and clinical determinants. Cerebrovasc Dis 2007; 23: 408–416.
    View article    Google Scholar
  5. Leśniak M, Bak T, Czepiel W, Seniów J, Członkowska A. Frequency and prognostic value of cognitive disorders in stroke patients. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2008; 26: 356–363.
    View article    Google Scholar
  6. Barker-Collo S, Feigin VL, Parag V, Lawes CMM, Senior H. Auckland Stroke Outcomes Study. Part 2: Cognition and functional outcomes 5 years poststroke. Neurology 2010; 75: 1608–1616.
    View article    Google Scholar
  7. Jokinen H, Melkas S, Ylikoski R, Pohjasvaara T, Kaste M, Erkinjuntti T, et al. Post-stroke cognitive impairment is common even after successful clinical recovery. Eur J Neurol 2015; 22: 1288–1294.
    View article    Google Scholar
  8. Patel MD, Coshall C, Rudd AG, Wolfe CDA. Cognitive impairment after stroke: clinical determinants and its associations with long-term stroke outcomes. J Am Geriatr Soc 2002; 50: 700–706.
    View article    Google Scholar
  9. Cumming TB, Tyedin K, Churilov L, Morris ME, Bernhardt J. The effect of physical activity on cognitive function after stroke: a systematic review. Int Psychogeriatr 2012; 24: 557–567.
    View article    Google Scholar
  10. García-Soto E, López de Munaín ML, Santibáñez M. [Effects of combined aerobic and resistance training on cognition following stroke: a systematic review.] Rev Neurol 2013; 57: 535–541. (in Spanish)
    View article    Google Scholar
  11. Kluding PM, Tseng BY, Billinger SA. Exercise and executive function in individuals with chronic stroke: a pilot study. J Neurol Phys Ther JNPT 2011; 35: 11–17.
    View article    Google Scholar
  12. Marzolini S, Oh P, McIlroy W, Brooks D. The effects of an aerobic and resistance exercise training program on cognition following stroke. Neurorehabil Neural Repair 2013; 27: 392–402.
    View article    Google Scholar
  13. Rand D, Eng JJ, Liu-Ambrose T, Tawashy AE. Feasibility of a 6-month exercise and recreation program to improve executive functioning and memory in individuals with chronic stroke. Neurorehabil Neural Repair 2010; 24: 722–729.
    View article    Google Scholar
  14. Quaney BM, Boyd LA, McDowd JM, Zahner LH, Jianghua He, Mayo MS, et al. Aerobic exercise improves cognition and motor function poststroke. Neurorehabil Neural Repair 2009; 23: 879–885.
    View article    Google Scholar
  15. Liu-Ambrose T, Eng JJ. Exercise training and recreational activities to promote executive functions in chronic stroke: a proof-of-concept study. J Stroke Cerebrovasc Dis 2015; 24: 130–137.
    View article    Google Scholar
  16. Tang A, Eng JJ, Krassioukov AV, Tsang TSM, Liu-Ambrose T. High- and low-intensity exercise do not improve cognitive function after stroke: a randomized controlled trial. J Rehabil Med 2016; 48: 841–846.
    View article    Google Scholar
  17. Oberlin LE, Waiwood AM, Cumming TB, Marsland AL, Bernhardt J, Erickson KI. Effects of physical activity on poststroke cognitive function: a meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Stroke 2017; 48: 3093–3100.
    View article    Google Scholar
  18. Heidari S, Babor TF, De Castro P, Tort S, Curno M. Sex and gender equity in research: rationale for the SAGER guidelines and recommended use. Res Integr Peer Rev 2016; 1: 2 eCollection 2016.
    View article    Google Scholar
  19. Haast RA, Gustafson DR, Kiliaan AJ. Sex differences in stroke. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 2012; 32: 2100–2107.
    View article    Google Scholar
  20. Appelros P, Stegmayr B, Terént A. Sex differences in stroke epidemiology: a systematic review. Stroke 2009; 40: 1082–1090.
    View article    Google Scholar
  21. Reeves MJ, Bushnell CD, Howard G, Gargano JW, Duncan PW, Lynch G, et al. Sex differences in stroke: epidemiology, clinical presentation, medical care, and outcomes. Lancet Neurol 2008; 7: 915–926.
    View article    Google Scholar
  22. Colcombe S, Kramer AF. Fitness effects on the cognitive function of older adults: a meta-analytic study. Psychol Sci 2003; 14: 125–130.
    View article    Google Scholar
  23. Barha CK, Galea LA, Nagamatsu LS, Erickson KI, Liu-Ambrose T. Personalising exercise recommendations for brain health: considerations and future directions. Br J Sports Med 2017; 51: 636–639.
    View article    Google Scholar
  24. Baker LD, Frank LL, Foster-Schubert K, Green PS, Wilkinson CW, McTiernan A, et al. Effects of aerobic exercise on mild cognitive impairment: a controlled trial. Arch Neurol 2010; 67: 71–79.
    View article    Google Scholar
  25. van Uffelen JGZ, Chinapaw MJM, van Mechelen W, Hopman-Rock M. Walking or vitamin B for cognition in older adults with mild cognitive impairment? A randomised controlled trial. Br J Sports Med 2008; 42: 344–351.
    View article    Google Scholar
  26. Barha CK, Davis JC, Falck RS, Nagamatsu LS, Liu-Ambrose T. Sex differences in exercise efficacy to improve cognition: a systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials in older humans. Front Neuroendocrinol 2017; 46 (Supplement C): 71–85.
    View article    Google Scholar
  27. Tang A, Eng JJ, Krassioukov AV, Madden KM, Mohammadi A, Tsang MYC, et al. Exercise-induced changes in cardiovascular function after stroke: a randomized controlled trial. Int J Stroke Off J Int Stroke Soc 2014; 9: 883–889.
    View article    Google Scholar
  28. Brott T, Adams HP, Olinger CP, Marler JR, Barsan WG, Biller J, et al. Measurements of acute cerebral infarction: a clinical examination scale. Stroke 1989; 20: 864–870.
    View article    Google Scholar
  29. Gowland C, Stratford P, Ward M, Moreland J, Torresin W, Van Hullenaar S, et al. Measuring physical impairment and disability with the Chedoke-McMaster Stroke Assessment. Stroke 1993; 24: 58–63.
    View article    Google Scholar
  30. ATS Committee on Proficiency Standards for Clinical Pulmonary Function Laboratories. ATS statement: guidelines for the six-minute walk test. Am J Respir Crit Care Med 2002 ; 166: 111–117.
    View article    Google Scholar
  31. Nasreddine ZS, Phillips NA, Bédirian V, Charbonneau S, Whitehead V, Collin I, et al. The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J Am Geriatr Soc 2005; 53: 695–699.
    View article    Google Scholar
  32. Damian AM, Jacobson SA, Hentz JG, Belden CM, Shill HA, Sabbagh MN, et al. The Montreal Cognitive Assessment and the mini-mental state examination as screening instruments for cognitive impairment: item analyses and threshold scores. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2011; 31: 126–131.
    View article    Google Scholar
  33. Radloff LS. The CES-D Scale: A Self-Report Depression Scale for Research in the General Population. Appl Psychol Meas 1977; 1: 385–401.
    View article    Google Scholar
  34. Wechsler D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale. 1997.
    View article    Google Scholar
  35. Reitan R, Wolfson D. The Halstead–Reitan Neuropsychological Test Battery: theory and clinical interpretation. Tucson, AZ: Neuropsychological Press; 1985.
    View article    Google Scholar
  36. Bowie CR, Harvey PD. Administration and interpretation of the Trail Making Test. Nat Protoc 2006; 1: 2277–2281.
    View article    Google Scholar
  37. Jensen AR, Rohwer Jr. WD. The Stroop Color-Word Test: a review. Acta Psychol (Amst) 1966; 25: 36–93.
    View article    Google Scholar
  38. Baum CM, Connor LT, Morrison T, Hahn M, Dromerick AW, Edwards DF. Reliability, validity, and clinical utility of the Executive Function Performance Test: a measure of executive function in a sample of people with stroke. Am J Occup Ther Off Publ Am Occup Ther Assoc 2008; 62: 446–455.
    View article    Google Scholar
  39. Goldstein G, Watson JR. Test-retest reliability of the Halstead-Reitan battery and the WAIS in a neuropsychiatric population. Clin Neuropsychol 1989; 3: 265–272.
    View article    Google Scholar
  40. Matarazzo JD, Wiens AN, Matarazzo RG, Goldstein SG. Psychometric and clinical test-retest reliability of the Halstead impairment index in a sample of healthy, young, normal men. J Nerv Ment Dis 1974; 158: 37–49.
    View article    Google Scholar
  41. Lindén T, Skoog I, Fagerberg B, Steen B, Blomstrand C. Cognitive impairment and dementia 20 months after stroke. Neuroepidemiology 2004; 23: 45–52.
    View article    Google Scholar
  42. American College of Sports Medicine. ACSM’s guidelines for exercise testing and prescription. 9th edition. Philadelphia, Lippincott Williams & Wilkins; 2013.
    View article    Google Scholar
  43. Enright PL, Sherrill DL. Reference equations for the six-minute walk in healthy adults. Am J Respir Crit Care Med 1998; 158: 1384–1387.
    View article    Google Scholar
  44. Nagamatsu LS, Handy TC, Hsu CL, Voss M, Liu-Ambrose T. Resistance training promotes cognitive and functional brain plasticity in seniors with probable mild cognitive impairment: a 6-month randomized controlled trial. Arch Intern Med 2012; 172: 666–668.
    View article    Google Scholar
  45. Liu-Ambrose T, Nagamatsu LS, Graf P, Beattie BL, Ashe MC, Handy TC. Resistance training and executive functions: a 12-month randomized controlled trial. Arch Intern Med 2010; 170: 170–178.
    View article    Google Scholar
  46. Banich MT. Executive function: the search for an integrated account. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 2009; 18: 89–94.
    View article    Google Scholar
  47. Ballard C, Stephens S, Kenny R, Kalaria R, Tovee M, O’Brien J. Profile of neuropsychological deficits in older stroke survivors without dementia. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2003; 16: 52–56.
    View article    Google Scholar
  48. Pohjasvaara T, Leskelä M, Vataja R, Kalska H, Ylikoski R, Hietanen M, et al. Post-stroke depression, executive dysfunction and functional outcome. Eur J Neurol 2002; 9: 269–275.
    View article    Google Scholar
  49. McCarrey AC, An Y, Kitner-Triolo MH, Ferrucci L, Resnick SM. Sex differences in cognitive trajectories in clinically normal older adults. Psychol Aging 2016; 31: 166–175.
    View article    Google Scholar
  50. Bo W, Lei M, Tao S, Jie LT, Qian L, Lin FQ, et al. Effects of combined intervention of physical exercise and cognitive training on cognitive function in stroke survivors with vascular cognitive impairment: a randomized controlled trial. Clin Rehabil 2019; 33; 54–63.
    View article    Google Scholar
  51. Triviño-Paredes J, Patten AR, Gil-Mohapel J, Christie BR. The effects of hormones and physical exercise on hippocampal structural plasticity. Front Neuroendocrinol 2016; 41: 23–43.
    View article    Google Scholar
  52. Hamson DK, Roes MM, Galea LAM. Sex Hormones and cognition: neuroendocrine influences on memory and learning. In: Comprehensive physiology. American Cancer Society; 2016 [cited 2018 Aug 31]. p. 1295–1337. Available from: https: //onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/abs/10.1002/cphy.c150031.
    View article    Google Scholar
  53. Galea LAM, Frick KM, Hampson E, Sohrabji F, Choleris E. Why estrogens matter for behavior and brain health. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2017; 76: 363–379.
    View article    Google Scholar
  54. Boss L, Kang D-H, Marcus M, Bergstrom N. Endogenous sex hormones and cognitive function in older adults: a systematic review. West J Nurs Res 2014; 36: 388–426.
    View article    Google Scholar
  55. LeBlanc ES, Wang PY, Janowsky JS, Neiss MB, Fink HA, Yaffe K, et al. Association between sex steroids and cognition in elderly men. Clin Endocrinol (Oxf) 2010; 72: 393–403.
    View article    Google Scholar
  56. Wolf OT, Kirschbaum C. Endogenous estradiol and testosterone levels are associated with cognitive performance in older women and men. Horm Behav 2002; 41: 259–266.
    View article    Google Scholar
  57. Phillips C, Baktir MA, Srivatsan M, Salehi A. Neuroprotective effects of physical activity on the brain: a closer look at trophic factor signaling. Front Cell Neurosci 2014; 8: 170.
    View article    Google Scholar
  58. Voss MW, Nagamatsu LS, Liu-Ambrose T, Kramer AF. Exercise, brain, and cognition across the life span. J Appl Physiol 2011; 111: 1505–1513.
    View article    Google Scholar
  59. Mackay CP, Kuys SS, Brauer SG. The Effect of Aerobic exercise on Brain-Derived Neurotrophic Factor in People with Neurological Disorders: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. [Cited 2018 Sep 16]. Neural Plast 2017; 2017: 4716197.
    View article    Google Scholar
  60. Szuhany KL, Bugatti M, Otto MW. A meta-analytic review of the effects of exercise on brain-derived neurotrophic factor. J Psychiatr Res 2015; 60: 56–64.
    View article    Google Scholar
  61. Komulainen P, Pedersen M, Hänninen T, Bruunsgaard H, Lakka TA, Kivipelto M, et al. BDNF is a novel marker of cognitive function in ageing women: the DR’s EXTRA Study. Neurobiol Learn Mem 2008; 90: 596–603.
    View article    Google Scholar
  62. Barha CK, Hsiung G-YR, Best JR, Davis JC, Eng JJ, Jacova C, et al. Sex difference in aerobic exercise efficacy to improve cognition in older adults with vascular cognitive impairment: secondary analysis of a randomized controlled trial. J Alzheimers Dis JAD 2017; 60: 1397–1410.
    View article    Google Scholar
  63. Sohrabji F, Miranda RC, Toran-Allerand CD. Identification of a putative estrogen response element in the gene encoding brain-derived neurotrophic factor. Proc Natl Acad Sci 1995; 92: 11110–11114.
    View article    Google Scholar
  64. Singh M, Meyer EM, Simpkins JW. The effect of ovariectomy and estradiol replacement on brain-derived neurotrophic factor messenger ribonucleic acid expression in cortical and hippocampal brain regions of female Sprague-Dawley rats. Endocrinology 1995; 136: 2320–2324.
    View article    Google Scholar
  65. Bakos J, Hlavacova N, Rajman M, Ondicova K, Koros C, Kitraki E, et al. Enriched environment influences hormonal status and hippocampal brain derived neurotrophic factor in a sex dependent manner. Neuroscience 2009; 164: 788–797.
    View article    Google Scholar
  66. Zhu S-W, Yee BK, Nyffeler M, Winblad B, Feldon J, Mohammed AH. Influence of differential housing on emotional behaviour and neurotrophin levels in mice. Behav Brain Res 2006; 169: 10–20.
    View article    Google Scholar
  67. Varma VR, Chuang Y-F, Harris GC, Tan EJ, Carlson MC. Low-intensity daily walking activity is associated with hippocampal volume in older adults. Hippocampus 2015; 25: 605–615.
    View article    Google Scholar
  68. Hasan SMM, Rancourt SN, Austin MW, Ploughman M. Defining optimal aerobic exercise parameters to affect complex motor and cognitive outcomes after stroke: a systematic review and synthesis. Neural Plast 2016; 2016: 2961573.
    View article    Google Scholar
  69. Stroke report. Heart and Stroke Foundation of Canada. 2018 [cited 2018 Sep 18]. Available from: http://www.heartandstroke.ca/what-we-do/media-centre/stroke-report.
    View article    Google Scholar

Comments

Do you want to comment on this paper? The comments will show up here and if appropriate the comments will also separately be forwarded to the authors. You need to login/create an account to comment on articles. Click here to login/create an account.