Content » Vol 48, Issue 2

Original report

Describing functioning and health after spinal cord injury in the light of psychological–personal factors

Szilvia Geyh, PhD1,2, Simon Kunz, MSc1,2, Rachel Müller, PhD1,2 and Claudio Peter, PhD1,2, for the SwiSCI Study Group

From the 1Swiss Paraplegic Research (SPF), Nottwil and 2Department of Health Sciences and Health Policy, University of Lucerne, Lucerne, Switzerland

OBJECTIVE: To describe and explore functioning and health of persons with spinal cord injury from the perspective of psychological-personal factors in the light of the International Classification of Functioning, Disability and Health (ICF) framework.

METHODS: Data from 511 participants regarding feelings, thoughts and beliefs, motives, and patterns of experience and behaviour were analysed. Measurement instruments included the Mental Health Index-5, Positive and Negative Affect Schedule, Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale, Appraisal of Life Events Scale, 5 items from the World Health Organization Quality of Life Scale, Purpose in Life Test-Short Form, General Self-Efficacy Scale, Big Five Inventory-21, Social Skills Inventory-SF, Brief COPE. The distribution of the selected psychological-personal factors-indicators was examined using descriptive statistics. Differences between SCI subgroups by sex, age, age at injury, time since injury, aetiology and severity of injury were explored using analysis of variance (ANOVA) and F-tests.

RESULTS: Participants who were older and sustained their spinal cord injury more recently experienced more depressed mood, less positive affect, less challenge appraisal, lower life satisfaction, lower purpose in life, and lower self-efficacy. They reported lower social skills, less usage of the coping strategies humour, positive reframing, and acceptance, and more usage of the coping strategies denial and self-distraction. Overall, effect sizes were small.

Discussion: Although study participants appeared to be well adjusted to spinal cord injury, those who sustained their injury at an older age and more recently reported more negative experiences. Quantitative description and exploration of the psychological-personal aspects of health will enable hypotheses to be formulated for further research, and suggest a need for tailored interventions for those at risk of less favourable outcomes.

Key words: spinal cord injury; International Classification of Functioning Disability and Health; psychological factors; personal factors.

J Rehabil Med 2016; 48: 219–234

Correspondence address: Szilvia Geyh, Swiss Paraplegic Research (SPF), CH-6207 Nottwil, Switzerland. E-mail: szilvia.geyh@paraplegie.ch

Accepted Aug 27, 2015; Epub ahead of print Feb 1, 2016

INTRODUCTION

Spinal cord injury (SCI) is a potentially life-threatening and profoundly life-changing health condition (1), which can have a significant impact on a person’s physical, psychological and social functioning. Complications, such as pain, bowel and bladder problems, muscle spasms, fatigue, osteoporosis and pressure sores, may occur (2). SCI may be associated with symptoms of depression and anxiety (3, 4), post-traumatic stress (5), substance abuse and suicidal behaviour (3). In addition, work, leisure and daily activities, relationships and participation (6–10) may change due to SCI.

Adjusting to the physical, psychological and social consequences of a SCI is a challenge for the affected individuals. Research shows that variations in adjustment to SCI are not well-predicted by biological factors, such as the severity of the injury or resulting impairments (11–13), implying that psychological and social factors might be better predictors.

Theory indicates that how an individual adjusts to an adverse event like SCI depends on their subjective experience and evaluation of the situation rather than only on the objective circumstances (14, 15). In other words, the individual’s beliefs, attitudes, expectations, emotional reactions, motivational structure and behavioural strategies can influence the adjustment process and are expected to determine health-related outcomes.

Empirical evidence on the role of individual psychological factors in adjusting to SCI has been summarized (3, 16–18). Findings indicate that cognitive appraisals significantly explain the variance in mental health (19) and psychological well-being (20, 21), but less consistently explain quality of life and life satisfaction (22). Persons with SCI and high self-efficacy (23–26) or purpose in life (27–29) report higher well-being and life satisfaction. Coping has been found to be related to mental health, participation and quality of life in SCI (3, 30); however, depending on the coping style, the results are inconsistent (22, 31, 32). Individuals with SCI who display stronger social skills (33–36), higher self-efficacy (37–44), higher emotional stability, agreeableness, extraversion, conscientiousness or openness show lower levels of depression and anxiety (45) and higher levels of participation (23, 46, 47).

The evidence on the role of individual psychological factors appears to be extensive, but contradictory, and only a few studies have considered the complex interrelations of the various components that may contribute to adjustment outcomes following SCI (31, 48–51). In addition, generalizations from research in SCI may be limited due to small sample sizes, sampling biases, low response rates, and the difficulty of comparing results due to the use of different operationalizations and measurements of psychological factors (17, 18).

However, individual psychological factors are important in order to comprehensively describe the functioning and health of individuals with SCI. Based on this description, research questions can be addressed, such as: “Who is at risk of negative outcomes and why?”, and “Which individual features relate how much to functioning?”. The answers to these questions can inform health professionals in developing interventions to enhance health, functioning and well-being of individuals with SCI.

Description of the full range of relevant factors builds the basis for a comprehensive understanding of functioning and health of individuals with SCI. The model of the World Health Organization’s International Classification of Functioning, Disability and Health (ICF) (52) posits interactions between dimensions of functioning (i.e. body, individual, social) and stresses the role of environmental and personal contextual factors as determinants of the extent of disability. The ICF can represent functioning and health of persons with SCI in a comprehensive manner from different perspectives, e.g. from a biomedical perspective, a social-environmental perspective, and a psychological–personal perspective.

A systematic approach to capture the individual psychological perspective on SCI in the light of the ICF has been proposed by Geyh (53). Based on this approach, the following 4 areas of psychological-personal factors (PPFs) can be considered for comprehensive understanding of individual functioning, health and well-being: Feelings, Thoughts and Beliefs, Motives, and Patterns of Experience and Behaviour. Presumably, various PPFs may be components, determinants, or even effects, of adjustment.

The overall objective of this paper is to describe and quantitatively explore functioning and health of persons with SCI with respect to these 4 areas of PPFs in the light of the ICF framework.

The specific aims of this paper are: (i) to report on the distribution of selected PPFs in the study population; and (ii) to explore differences in PPFs between SCI subgroups by sex, age, age at injury, time since injury, aetiology and severity of injury.

METHODS

This study is part of a nationwide community-based cross-sectional survey conducted within the Swiss Spinal Cord Injury Cohort Study (SwiSCI) (54, 55). The SwiSCI was conducted in collaboration with the 4 specialized SCI rehabilitation clinics in Switzerland, including the Spinal Cord Injury Center of Balgrist University Hospital, Zürich; the Centre for Spinal Cord Injury and Severe Head Injury, REHAB Basel; the Clinique Romande de Réadaptation CRR, Sion; and the Swiss Paraplegic Centre (SPZ), Nottwil. Further participating organizations include the Swiss Paraplegic Association (SPV); the home nursing service organization ParaHelp; and the Swiss Accident Insurance Corporation (SUVA). SwiSCI received approval from the cantonal ethics committees.

SwiSCI aimed to include all community-dwelling persons aged 16 years or older with permanent residence in Switzerland and a diagnosis of traumatic or non-traumatic SCI. SwiSCI excluded persons with congenital conditions leading to para- or tetraplegia, including spina bifida, new SCI in the context of palliative (end-of-life) care, neurodegenerative disorders including multiple sclerosis (MS) and amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (ALS), or Guillain-Barré syndrome.

Data were collected in 3 waves from September 2011 to March 2013. In the first wave, study information, informed consent form and a first questionnaire assessing sociodemographic and lesion-related characteristics were mailed to all eligible persons. Persons who returned a signed consent form and the first questionnaire received a second-wave questionnaire about health, functioning, participation and well-being. With protections of privacy and following the ethical protocol, the collaborating organisations were able to provide the study centre with anonymized data about the non-participants, including date of injury, age, sex, type of lesion and language, which made it possible to investigate non-response bias (56). In the third wave, 3 randomized samples, stratified by sex, age, and level of lesion (para- vs. tetraplegia) were drawn from the respondents of the second-wave questionnaire. Each of the 3 samples received a different module. PPFs were assessed in the Psychological Personal Factors and Health Behaviour (PPFs-HB) module of the third wave (54).

SwiSCI initially contacted 3144 potential participants for data collection. Response rate in the first wave questionnaire was 61.4%, in the second 82.6%, and in the third 87.6%, which resulted in the present sample of n = 511. As shown in Table I, 27% of the sample was female. Mean age was 52.9 years and participants had lived for a mean of 17 years with paraplegia (69%) or tetraplegia (31%)

Table I. Descriptive characteristics of study participants (n = 511)

Characteristic

n (%)

Mean (SD)

Median

n (%)

missing

Sex

 

 

 

 

Male

371 (72.6)

 

 

 

Female

140 (27.4)

 

 

 

Age, years

 

52.9 (14.8)

53

 

Age at injury, years

 

35.4 (17.6)

32

5 (1.0)

Time since injury, years

 

17.45 (13.06)

14

5 (1.0)

Age at injury and time since injury groups

 

 

 

5 (1.0)

Younger at and shorter time since injury

61 (11.9)

 

 

 

Younger at and longer time since injury

189 (37.0)

 

 

 

Older at and shorter time since injury

192 (37.6)

 

 

 

Older at and longer time since injury

64 (12.5)

 

 

 

Type of lesion

 

 

 

3 (0.5)

Complete paraplegia

166 (32.5)

 

 

 

Complete tetraplegia

56 (11.0)

 

 

 

Incomplete paraplegia

184 (36.0)

 

 

 

Incomplete tetraplegia

102 (20.0)

 

 

 

Cause of injury

 

 

 

2 (0.4)

Traumatic

400 (78.3)

 

 

 

Non-traumatic

109 (21.3)

 

 

 

Education, years

 

13.8 (3.3)

 

12 (2.4)

Marital status

 

 

 

5 (1.0)

Single (never married)

158 (30.9)

 

 

 

Married

255 (49.9)

 

 

 

Widowed

68 (13.3)

 

 

 

Divorced

23 (4.5)

 

 

 

Registered Partnership

2 (0.4)

 

 

 

Language of questionnaire

 

 

 

 

German

361 (70.7)

 

 

 

French

129 (25.2)

 

 

 

Italian

21 (4.1)

 

 

 

SD: standard deviation.

16220.png
16222.png

.

Variables and instruments

Sociodemographic, injury-related and information on PPFs were assessed. Sociodemographic characteristics included age, sex, years of education, marital status, and language of the questionnaire. Injury-related information collected was: age at injury, time since injury, type of lesion (complete/incomplete, para/tetraplegia), cause of injury (traumatic/non-traumatic). The ICF-based framework by Geyh (53) was used to capture PPFs areas, including Feelings, Thoughts and Beliefs, Motives, Patterns of Experience and Behaviour. Validated German, French and Italian versions of the measures were used. French and Italian versions of the Purpose in Life Test-Short Form (PIL-SF) and the Appraisal of Life Events Scale (ALE) were translated according to World Health Organization (WHO) guidelines (57) by the SwiSCI team. The following self-report measures were used to assess PPFs (Table II).

Table II. Concept areas to capture psychological-personal factors (PPFs) in the context of the International Classification of Functioning, Disability and Health (ICF) framework and according to measurement instruments

PPFs area

Measurement instrument

Abbreviation

Items

Time-frame

Subscales

Items per subscale

Scoring

Score interpretation

Feelings

Mental Health Index (58)

MHI-5

5

Past 4 weeks

Transformed sum score

0: low mood/high distress

100: high mood/low distress

Positive And Negative Affect Schedule (60)

PANAS

20

Last week

Positive Affect (PA)

Negative Affect (NA)

10

Sum score

10: less positive/less negative affect

50: more positive/more negative affect

Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale – Depression subscale (63)

HADS-D

7

Last week

 

7

Sum score

0: no depressed feelings

21: highly depressed feelings

Thoughts and Beliefs

Appraisal of Life Events Scale (ALE) (66)

ALE

16

Past 3 months

Challenge

Loss

Threat

6

4

6

Sum score

0: low degree of appraisal

30 (20): high degree of appraisal

5 items from the World Health Organisation Quality of Life Scale (70, 71)

5 WHOQoL

5

Past 2 weeks

Mean score

1: dissatisfied with life

5: satisfied with life

Motives

Purpose in Life Test-Short Form (73)

PIL-SF

4

Now

Sum score

4: no purpose in life

28: strong purpose in life

Patterns

of Experience

and Behavior

General Self-Efficacy Scale (75)

GSES

10

In general

Sum score

10: low general self-efficacy

40: high general self-efficacy

Big Five Inventory-21 (79)

BFI-21

21

In general

Openness

Conscientiousness

Extraversion

Agreeableness

Neuroticism

5

4

4

4

4

Mean score

1: low level of personality trait

5: high level of personality trait

Social Skills Inventory – Short Form (80, 81)

SSI-SF

30

In general

Expressivity

Sensitivity

Control

10

Sum score

10: low level of social skill

50: high level of social skill

Brief COPE (84)

COPE

28

In general

Self-distraction

Active coping

Denial

Substance use

Emotional support

Instrumental support

Behavioral disengagement

Venting

Positive reframing

Planning

Humor

Acceptance

Religion

Self-blame

2

Sum score

 

2: coping strategy is not used

8: coping strategy is used a lot

 

 

Feelings

The PPF area Feelings was addressed using the following 3 measures related to feelings and mood.

Mental Health Index. The MHI-5 is 1 of 8 scales included in the 36-item Short Form health survey (SF-36) (58). The subscale consists of 5 items assessing frequency of feeling nervous, down in the dumps, calm and peaceful, downhearted and blue, and happy. Responses are given on a 5-point Likert scale (“all” to “none of the time”). Evidence concerning validity and reliability supports the use of the MHI-5 in individuals with disabilities (59). In contrast to the other measures of PPFs, the MHI was assessed in the second wave of data collections.

Positive and Negative Affect Schedule. The PANAS (60) is a 20-item measure of positive affect (PANAS-PA) and negative affect (PANAS-NA) (61). Respondents are asked to rate the extent to which they have experienced 10 particular positive (e.g. enthusiasm) and 10 negative emotions (e.g. distress) on a 5-point Likert scale (“very slightly or not at all” to “very much”). The PANAS has evidence supporting its validity and reliability in a rehabilitation population (62).

Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale. HADS-D is a 7-item depression subscale of the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (63) that assesses mood and affect-related (but not somatic) symptoms of depression. Responses are given on a 0–3 Likert scale, with higher scores indicating more severe depressive symptoms. The HADS-D has been found to be reliable and valid in measuring symptom severity of depression in individuals with SCI (64). Scores between 8 and 10 are considered as mild cases, 11–15 moderate cases, and 16 or above severe cases of depression (63, 65).

Thoughts and Beliefs

The PPF area Thoughts and Beliefs was addressed by 2 measures of cognitive appraisal and evaluations of life’s challenges and quality:

Appraisal of Life Events Scale. The ALE (66) consists of 16 adjectives that represent cognitive appraisals of stressful life-events on 3 dimensions: threat (e.g. “terrifying”), challenge (e.g. “stimulating”) and loss (e.g. “depressing”). Respondents rate the extent to which the adjectives describe their concerns on a 6-point Likert scale (“not at all” to “very much so”). The ALE has been frequently applied in SCI population (67, 68) and was found to be reliable and valid in measuring appraisal in the general population (66, 69).

WHO Quality of Life Scale. Five selected items of the WHOQoL-BREF ask respondents to rate their overall quality of life, satisfaction with health, daily activities, relationships, and living conditions on a 5-point response scale. The mean score is used for analyses. The selected items were found to be reliable and valid to assess quality of life in individuals with SCI (70–72).

Motives

The PPF area Motives was represented by the Purpose in Life Test-Short Form (PIL-SF) (73, 74). Respondents are asked to answer 4 questions regarding their life goals, purpose and their progress in achieving life goals on a 7-step rating scale. The PIL-SF demonstrated good reliability and validity (73).

Patterns of Experience and Behavior

The PPF area Patterns of Experience and Behavior was represented using the following 4 measures of thought patterns, traits, skills and strategies.

General Self-Efficacy Scale. The GSE consists of 10 items assessing a broad and stable sense of personal competence to deal with stressful situations (75) rated on a 4-point Likert scale. The GSE was found reliable and valid (75–77) and has been frequently used in SCI research (78).

Big Five Inventory. The 21-item BFI-21 (79) measures the personality traits extraversion, neuroticism, agreeableness, conscientiousness and openness to experiences. Respondents are asked to indicate the extent to which they agree or disagree with 21 statements about their personality characteristics. A 5-point Likert scale (“strongly disagree” to “strongly agree”) is used. The BFI-21 has been found reliable and valid in assessing personality traits (79).

Social Skills Inventory – Short Form. The SSI-SF (80, 81) assesses elements of social skills (i.e. expressivity, sensitivity, control). Responses are given on a 5-point Likert-Scale (“not at all like me” to “exactly like me”). The original SSI has been found to have good content validity and acceptable internal consistency in the general population (82).

Brief COPE. The BriefCOPE (83) is an abbreviated version of the COPE Inventory (84) and measures ways of coping with problems and stress. It consists of 28 items that represent 14 subscales (self-distraction, active coping, denial, substance use, use of emotional support, use of instrumental support, behavioural disengagement, venting, positive reframing, planning, humour, acceptance, religion, self-blame). Higher scores refer to higher frequency of using a specific coping strategy. The COPE is frequently used in the SCI population (e.g. 83). Reliability and validity have been found acceptable (83, 86).

Analyses

To report on the distribution of the selected PPFs in the SwiSCI population, measures of central tendency (mean and median) and measures of dispersion (standard deviation (SD), 1st and 3rd quartiles) were calculated using sum scores of all the measures. To evaluate the shape of the statistical distributions of the scores, kurtosis and skewness, the percentage of minimum and maximum scorers in the sample were calculated, and the normality of the distributions was tested using Shapiro–Wilk test (alpha-error level 0.01). Kurtosis is mesokurtic at k = 3, leptokurtic or peaked with k > 3, and platykurtic or flat with k < 3 (87). Negative skewness suggests a left-tailed distribution with values concentrated on the right side of the score range, while a positive skewness suggests a right-tailed distribution with values concentrated on the left side of the continuum.

The distributions were examined graphically by creating and comparing frequency graphs for the questionnaire scores by categorical population variables sex, type of lesion and cause of injury. For the continuous variables (age, age at injury, time since injury) scatterplots were created.

To explore differences between subgroups, differences in questionnaire scores were tested by sex, age, age at injury, time since injury, aetiology and severity of injury. For the continuous variables age, age at injury and time since injury the median split was used to create subgroups of approximately equal size. Time since injury and age at injury were combined as they are interdependent variables on the same dimension, time. Analyses of variance (ANOVA) were conducted to detect overall differences between the subgroups. To address multiple testing Bonferroni correction was applied. Since the same questionnaire data are tested for differences 7 times, namely for the 7 subgroup variables, we correct the alpha level 0.05/7 = 0.007. ANOVA effect size is regarded as small with an eta-square around 0.01, medium around 0.059 and large around 0.138 (88). Pearson coefficients for correlations between continuous variables age, age at injury and time since injury and questionnaire scores were reported. The contrasting subgroups were identified using confidence intervals.

The requirements for ANOVA were tested according to normality (Shapiro–Wilk test) and homoscedasticity (Levene’s test, alpha=0.01) of the distributions. While ANOVA is robust against violations of normal distribution (89), heteroscedasticity was accounted for by using logarithmic transformation. If logarithmic transformation did not remedy heteroscedasticity, non-parametric procedures were used to examine subgroup differences. For dichotomous data the Mann-Whitney test is used, reporting z and p-value. For polytomous data the Kruskal–Wallis test is used, reporting χ2, df and p.

To address missingness within the data-set, non-parametric missing value imputation using random forest was applied with the “missForest” package in R. In addition, systematic non-response bias in SwiSCI was accounted for by using inverse probability weighting based on propensity scores from multivariate logistic regression referring to the source population of n = 3,144 (for more details see (56)). Non-response in the SwiSCI study was explained by membership in the Swiss Paraplegic Association and time since injury. Non-response was not related to age, sex, type of lesion or language.

Analyses were conducted with STATA (90) and R (91). However, the STATA program used for calculating the propensity scores does not support non-parametric analyses for weighted data. Therefore, we report non-parametric analyses for unweighted data and use the Pearson correlation coefficient throughout.

RESULTS

Distribution of selected Psycohological Personal Factors in the SwiSCI population (Table III)

Feelings. With the exception of the positive affect subscale, the 4 measures used as indicators of feelings, mood and affect were not normally distributed in the present sample (p < 0.01). The MHI-5 and the PANAS-PA show negative left-skewed and platykurtic flat distributions. The MHI-5 had a median (Md) = 76 with an interquartile range (IQR) = 28 on a 0–100 scale, suggesting moderately high mood. PANAS-PA had a Md = 32 and IQR = 10 on a 10–50 scale, suggesting moderately strong positive affect in the past week.

The PANAS-NA and the HADS-D showed right-skewed and peaked distributions. Scores on the PANAS-NA (Md = 17, IQR = 8) and HADS-D (Md = 4, IQR=5) suggested little negative affect and depressed feelings in the sample overall in the past week. In both scales, a slight floor effect was detected. Mild depressive symptoms were reported by 13.1% of the sample, 6.8% reported experiencing moderate and 1.4% severe depressive symptoms.

Table III. Distributions of the different psychological-personal factors (PPFs)

PPFs Area

Measure

Subscale

Missing

n (%)

Cronbach‘s

α

Mean [CI]

Median

SD

25% Quartile

75% Quartile

Maximum

n (%)

Minimum

n (%)

Kurtosis

Skewness

pa

Feelings

MHI-5

 

40 (7.8)

0.86

72.4 [70.8–74.0]

76

18.1

60

88

14 (2.8)

0

2.82

–0.72

0.000

PANAS

Positive affect

19 (3.7)

0.90

32.0 [31.4–32.7]

32

7.4

27

37

1 (0.2)

0

2.70

–0.20

0.028

PANAS

Negative affect

17 (3.3)

0.86

18.0 [17.5–18.6]

17

6.3

13

21

0

38 (7.4)

3.99

1.02

0.000

HADS-D

 

10 (2.0)

0.84

4.7 [4.3–5.0]

4

3.9

2

7

0

55 (10.7)

3.90

1.05

0.000

Thoughts

ALE

Challenge

35 (6.9)

0.87

12.0 [11.4–12.7]

12

7.4

6

18

0

49 (9.7)

2.02

–0.01

0.000

ALE

Loss

32 (6.3)

0.83

6.8 [6.3–7.2]

6

5.1

3

11

6 (1.1)

66 (12.9)

2.43

0.49

0.000

ALE

Threat

34 (6.7)

0.87

9.0 [8.4–9.6]

8

6.9

3

14

1 (0.2)

57 (11.1)

2.44

0.51

0.000

5 WHOQoL

 

4 (0.8)

0.80

3.3 [3.3–3.4]

3.8

0.7

3.40

4.20

16 (3.2)

1 (0.2)

3.34

–0.63

0.000

Motives

PIL-SF

 

11 (2.2)

0.89

21.2 [20.8–21.6]

22

4.7

18

25

23 (4.5)

2 (0.3)

3.58

–0.90

0.000

Patterns of

Experience

and

Behavior

GSES

 

13 (2.5)

0.91

30.3 [29.8–30.8]

30

5.6

28

34

18 (3.5)

1 (0.2)

3.54

–0.64

0.000

BFI-21

Openness

9 (1.8)

0.70

3.8 [3.7–3.8]

3.8

0.7

3.20

4.20

28 (5.5)

0

3.09

–0.27

0.000

BFI-21

Conscientiousness

9 (1.8)

0.63

3.8 [3.8–3.9]

3.75

0.6

3.50

4.25

31 (6.1)

0

3.03

–0.27

0.013

BFI-21

Extraversion

9 (1.8)

0.70

3.4 [3.4–3.5]

3.5

0.8

3

4

15 (2.9)

2 (0.3)

2.94

–0.24

0.258

BFI-21

Agreeableness

9 (1.8)

0.59

3.6 [3.5–3.7]

3.5

0.7

3

4

13 (2.6)

0

2.76

–0.19

0.044

BFI-21

Neuroticism

10 (2.0)

0.76

2.5 [2.5–2.6]

2.5

0.9

2

3

1 (0.2)

21 (4.1)

2.50

0.20

0.001

SSI-SF

Expressivity

16 (3.1)

0.72

30.5 [30.0–31.0]

30

5.9

26

34

1 (0.2)

0

3.23

0.11

0.625

SSI-SF

Sensitivity

21 (4.1)

0.72

28.6 [28.1–29.1]

28

5.6

25

32

0

0

3.22

0.09

0.185

SSI-SF

Control

17 (3.3)

0.69

33.5 [33.0–34.0]

34

5.5

30

37

0

0

3.50

–0.10

0.029

COPE

Self-distraction

7 (1.4)

0.47

4.9 [4.8–5.0]

5

1.6

4

6

28 (5.4)

42 (8.3)

2.44

–0.01

1.000

COPE

Active coping

6 (1.2)

0.71

6.0 [5.9–6.1]

6

1.5

5

7

103 (20.1)

9 (1.7)

2.61

–0.42

0.005

COPE

Denial

9 (1.8)

0.55

3.3 [3.1–3.4]

3

1.5

2

4

8 (1.6)

229 (44.9)

3.72

1.14

0.000

COPE

Substance use

7 (1.4)

0.92

2.6 [2.4–2.7]

2

1.2

2

3

7 (1.3)

380 (74.4)

9.19

2.45

0.000

COPE

Emotional support

7 (1.4)

0.78

4.1 [3.9–4.2]

4

1.6

3

5

20 (3.9)

103 (20.2)

2.81

0.60

0.000

COPE

Instrumental support

6 (1.2)

0.77

4.4 [4.3–4.5]

4

1.5

4

5

20 (4.0)

65 (12.6)

2.86

0.36

0.004

COPE

Behavioral disengagement

8 (1.6)

0.42

3.1 [3.0–3.2]

3

1.3

2

4

4 (0.7)

249 (48.7)

3.74

1.09

0.000

COPE

Venting

7 (1.4)

0.60

3.9 [3.8–4.0]

4

1.4

3

5

6 (1.1)

108 (21.1)

2.67

0.45

0.000

COPE

Positive reframing

7 (1.4)

0.62

5.6 [5.5–5.7]

6

1.6

4

7

83 (16.3)

14 (2.8)

2.24

–0.13

0.298

COPE

Planning

7 (1.4)

0.50

5.3 [5.2–5.4]

6

1.5

4

6

33 (6.4)

27 (5.3)

2.72

–0.32

0.076

COPE

Humor

11 (2.2)

0.69

4.0 [3.9–4.2]

4

1.7

3

5

16 (3.2)

121 (23.8)

2.40

0.48

0.001

COPE

Acceptance

12 (2.4)

0.71

6.4 [6.3–6.5]

6

1.5

6

8

157 (30.8)

6 (1.2)

3.03

–0.74

0.000

COPE

Religion

12 (2.4)

0.89

3.7 [3.5–3.9]

3

2.0

2

5

38 (7.5)

221 (43.3)

2.59

0.91

0.000

COPE

Self-blame

7 (1.4)

0.58

3.7 [3.6–3.8]

4

1.5

2

5

8 (1.6)

152 (29.7)

2.68

0.62

0.000

Missing refers to missing total scores. ap: p-value of the Shapiro–Wilk test for normal distribution. Cronbach’s alpha and p-value of the Shapiro–Wilk test are based on unweighted data.

CI: 95% confidence interval; SD: standard deviation; MHI-5: Mental Health Index; PANAS: Positive And Negative Affect Schedule; HADS-D: Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale – Depression subscale; ALE: Appraisal of Life Events Scale; 5 WHOQoL: 5 items from the World Health Organisation Quality of Life Scale; PIL-SF: Purpose in Life Test-Short Form; GSES: General Self-Efficacy Scale; BFI-21: Big Five Inventory-21; SSI-SF: Social Skills Inventory - Short Form; COPE: Brief COPE.

Thoughts and Beliefs. The scores in the 4 scales used as indicators of the cognitive evaluation of the subjects’ own situation were not normally distributed. The distribution of ALE-Challenge evaluations was not skewed, but flat, with a Md = 12 and IQR = 12 on a 0–30 scale and 9.7% of the sample scoring 0, suggesting overall that participants interpret their situation to a low to moderate degree in terms of being a challenge. The ALE subscales Loss (Md = 6, IQR = 8, scale 0–20) and Threat (Md = 8; IQR = 11, scale 0–30) were both platykurtic and right-skewed, showing considerable floor effects, with 12.9% and 11.1% of participants scoring 0, which indicates that they interpreted their situation to a low degree as loss or threat.

The distribution of the 5 WHOQoL items was skewed to the left and leptokurtic with a Md = 3.8 and IQR = 0.80 on a 1–5 scale, indicating that, on average, participants were satisfied with different domains of their lives.

Motives. A non-normal distribution of the PIL-SF scores was found in the present sample. The distribution was left-skewed and leptokurtic. The median sum score is 22 with an IQR = 7 on a scale with theoretical minimum of 4 and maximum of 28. This indicates that participants overall report having rather clear life goals, perceiving a strong purpose and meaning in life.

Patterns of Experience and Behavior. Only scores of 1 indicator of Patterns of Experience and Behavior was not normally distributed in the present sample (p < 0.01). The GSES scores were left-skewed and leptokurtic, with Md=30 and IQR = 6, indicating that overall participants experience a moderately strong general self-efficacy.

The subscales Conscientiousness, Extraversion and Agreeableness of the BFI-21 are normally distributed. The scores in BFI-21 subscales Openness (M = 3.75, SD = 0.69), Conscientiousness (M = 3.84, SD = 0.67), Extraversion (M = 3.44, SD = 0.76) and Agreeableness (M = 3.60, SD = 0.71) demonstrated that participants located themselves in the higher middle range of these personality characteristics. The distribution of the subscale Neuroticism (Md = 2.5, IQR = 1) was slightly skewed to the right, platykurtic with a considerable floor effect and 24.2% of the sample scoring at the minimum. This indicates overall that participants rather did not perceive themselves as nervous, tense or worried personalities.

All 3 subscales of the SSI-SF were normally distributed. On a scale of 10–50, participants showed a tendency to locate themselves in the middle range of the social skill characteristics Expressivity (M = 30.47, SD = 5.87), Sensitivity (M = 28.58, SD = 5.62) and Control (M = 33.48, SD = 5.46).

Among the 14 subscales of the COPE, 3 were found to be normally distributed on a scale ranging from 2 to 8: Self-distraction (M = 4.90, SD = 1.56), Positive reframing (M = 5.59, SD = 1.60) and Planning (M = 5.31, SD = 1.48). Overall, these strategies seem to be applied by the present sample with medium frequency. Active coping (Md = 6, IQR = 2) and Acceptance (Md = 6, IQR = 2) were 2 subscales with left-skewed distributions displaying strong ceiling effects. The maximum score of 8 was achieved by 20.1% of the participants in the use of Active coping and by 30.8% in the use of Acceptance. The subscales Emotional support (Md = 4, IQR = 2), Instrumental support (Md = 4, IQR = 1), Venting (Md = 4, IQR = 2), Humor (Md = 4, IQR = 2) and Self-blame (Md = 4, IQR = 3) were reported overall to be applied with low frequency. The distributions of these subscales are right-skewed and platykurtic and show considerable floor effects, i.e. the strategies were reported to be never used by 12.6% to 29.7% of the participants. The coping strategies Denial (Md = 3, IRQ = 2), Substance use (Md = 2, IRQ = 1), Behavioural disengagement (Md = 3, IRQ = 2) and Religion (Md = 3, IRQ = 3) were reported overall to be applied very seldom or not at all. The distribution of these subscales is right-skewed and peaked, except Religion, which is platykurtic. The minimum score of 2, i.e. no use of this strategy at all, is achieved by 43.3–74.4% of the participants.

Differences in Psycohological Personal Factors between SCI subgroups

Feelings. In the PPFs area of Feelings the analyses did not detect differences within the sample with regards to sex type of lesion or cause of injury (Table IV). The MHI-5 and the PANAS-NA did not vary systematically with any of the analysed population variables. The PANAS-PA, however, showed small, but statistically significant, differences regarding age, age at injury (AaI), time since injury (TsI) and in the combined AaI and TsI subgroups. Younger participants with SCI reported slightly more positive affect than older participants. Also, those participants with a younger AaI reported slightly more positive affect than those older at injury. Those who have been living longer with SCI reported slightly more positive affect than those with shorter TsI. The combined AaI and TsI groups differed in the experience of positive affect: participants who were older at injury and were injured more recently reported having less positive affect.

Table IV. Subgroup differences in measurement scores in the areas Feelings, Thoughts and Beliefs, and Motives

Population variable

Statistic

MHI-5

PANAS-PA

PANAS-NA

HADS-D

ALE Challenge

ALE Loss

ALE Threat

5 WHOQoL

PIL-SF

Sex

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Male

Mean [CI]

73.78 [71.96–75.59]

31.88 [31.15–32.62]

17.66 [17.06–18.26]

4.67 [4.27–5.07]

12.28 [11.52–13.04]

6.35 [5.86–6.84]

8.76 [8.09–9.42]

3.74 [3.67–3.81]

21.25 [20.76–21.73]

Female

Mean [CI]

69.12 [66.02–72.21]

32.42 [31.13–33.70]

18.88 [17.69–20.07]

4.68 [4.01–5.35]

11.44 [10.21–12.66]

7.82 [6.89–8.76]

9.71 [8.41–11.01]

3.73 [3.61–3.85]

21.14 [20.38–21.90]

 

F, df, p

7.10, 1, 0.008

0.56, 1, 0.456

4.05, 1, 0.045

0.00, 1, 0.977

1.39,1, 0.239

6.59, 1, 0.011a

2.02, 1, 0.156

0.01, 1, 0.927

0.06, 1, 0.814

 

η2

0.014

0.001

0.008

0.000

0.003

0.015

0.004

0.000

0.000

Age

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Overall

r

0.03

–0.14**

–0.11*

0.14**

–0.27***

–0.05

–0.07

–0.08

–0.11**

Younger

Mean [CI]

72.12 [69.85–74.38]

33.06 [32.13–33.99]

18.60 [17.78–19.41]

4.31 [3.85–4.78]

13.71 [12.86–14.55]

7.14 [6.54–7.74]

9.58 [8.73–10.43]

3.78 [3.70–3.87]

21.77 [21.24–22.31]

Older

Mean [CI]

72.70 [70.49–74.90]

30.99 [30.13–31.86]

17.43 [16.70–18.15]

5.05 [4.55–5.55]

10.32 [9.39–11.24]

6.42 [5.78–7.07]

8.48 [7.63–9.33]

3.69 [3.61–3.78]

20.64 [20.04–21.25]

 

F, df, p

0.13, 1, 0.719

10.25, 1, 0.002

4.45, 1, 0.035

4.53, 1, 0.034

4,43, 0.000b

2.56, 1, 0.110

3.25, 1, 0.072

2.20, 1, 0.138

2.24, 0.025b

 

η2

0.000

0.020

0.009

0.009

 

0.005

0.006

0.004

 

Age at injury

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Overall

r

–0.03

–0.18***

–0.03

0.24***

–0.33***

0.04

–0.01

–0.18***

–0.19***

Younger

Mean [CI]

74.17 [72.09–76.26]

33.41 [32.54–34.29]

17.85 [17.10–18.60]

3.76 [3.35–4.16]

14.08 [13.23–14.94]

6.48 [5.91–7.04]

9.02 [8.20–9.83]

3.86 [3.77–3.94]

22.22 [21.74–22.71]

Older

Mean [CI]

70.85 [68.51–73.19]

30.84 [29.93–31.74]

18.16 [17.37–18.96]

5.48 [4.96–6.00]

10.23 [9.33–11.13]

7.06 [6.39–7.73]

9.06 [8.17–9.94]

3.63 [3.55–3.72]

20.33 [19.70–20.96]

 

F, df, p

1.92, 0.055b

16.07, 1, 0.000

0.31, 1, 0.576

13.77, 1, 0.000a

36.96, 1, 0.000

3.38, 1, 0.067a

0.00, 1, 0.949

13.29, 1, 0.000

3.99, 0.000b

 

η2

 

0.030

0.001

0.030

0.068

0.008

0.000

0.025

 

Table IV. Contd.

Population variable

Statistic

MHI-5

PANAS-PA

PANAS-NA

HADS-D

ALE Challenge

ALE Loss

ALE Threat

5 WHOQoL

PIL-SF

Sex

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Time since injury

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Overall

r

0.08

0.10*

–0.08

–0.17***

0.14**

–0.12**

–0.07

0.17***

0.13**

Shorter

Mean [CI]

70.79 [68.40–73.17]

31.19 [30.25–32.13]

18.52 [17.74–19.31]

5.35 [4.83–5.88]

11.01 [10.10–11.92]

7.29 [6.64–7.95]

9.37 [8.50–10.24]

3.63 [3.54–3.72]

20.60 [19.95–21.24]

Longer

Mean [CI]

74.16 [72.12–76.20]

32.97 [32.11–33.83]

17.47 [16.71–18.22]

3.94 [3.52–4.36]

13.14 [12.24–14.04]

6.23 [5.65–6.81]

8.68 [7.84–9.51]

3.86 [3.78–3.93]

21.89 [21.41–22.37]

 

F, df, p

–1.88, 0.060b

7.54, 1, 0.006

3.63, 1, 0.057

9.35, 1, 0.002a

10.76, 1, 0.001

7.13, 1, 0.008a

1.28, 1, 0.259

13.82, 1, 0.000

–2.46, 0.014b

 

η2

 

0.015

0.007

0.020

0.021

0.016

0.003

0.026

 

Combined

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Younger shorter

Mean [CI]

74.93 [70.63–79.23]

33.46 [31.69–35.24]

19.07 [17.53–20.61]

4.06 [3.16–4.96]

14.42 [12.66–16.19]

6.42 [5.29–7.56]

8.87 [7.21–10.52]

3.85 [3.67–4.03]

22.33 [21.24–23.41]

Younger longer

Mean [CI]

73.90 [71.50–76.30]

33.40 [32.38–34.42]

17.40 [16.55–18.26]

3.65 [3.20–4.10]

13.96 [12.97–14.95]

6.49 [5.84–7.15]

9.07 [8.12–10.02]

3.86 [3.77–3.95]

22.19 [21.65–22.72]

Older shorter

Mean [CI]

69.48 [66.65–72.30]

30.47 [29.38–31.55]

18.35 [17.43–19.27]

5.76 [5.14–6.38]

9.93 [8.91–10.95]

7.57 [6.78–8.36]

9.53 [8.51–10.55]

3.56 [3.46–3.66]

20.05 [19.29–20.81]

Older longer

Mean [CI]

74.81 [70.85–78.77]

31.90 [30.28–33.51]

17.62 [16.02–19.22]

4.66 [3.69–5.63]

11.09 [9.17–13.01]

5.57 [4.36–6.79]

7.69 [5.94–9.44]

3.85 [3.70–4.00]

21.14 [20.10–22.18]

 

F, df, p

6.85, 3, 0.077b

6.03, 3, 0.001

1.44, 3, 0.231

5.31, 3, 0.001a

12.83, 3, 0.000

3.14, 3, 0.025a

1.24, 3, 0.295

7.61, 3, 0.000

18.09, 3, 0.000b

 

η2

 

0.034

0.008

0.034

0.071

0.021

0.007

0.043

 

Type of lesion

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Complete paraplegia

Mean [CI]

73.92 [71.28–76.56]

33.13 [32.00–34.26]

17.78 [16.77–18.78]

3.99 [3.45–4.52]

13.10 [11.90–14.30]

6.19 [5.43–6.96]

8.39 [7.33–9.46]

3.88 [3.77–3.99]

21.82 [21.02–22.44]

Complete tetraplegia

Mean [CI]

74.11 [69.71–78.51]

31.23 [29.28–33.19]

17.44 [15.86–19.03]

4.43 [3.35–5.51]

11.82 [10.23–13.40]

7.03 [5.81–8.24]

10.25 [8.42–12.07]

3.64 [3.45–3.82]

21.17 [19.91–22.43]

Incomplete paraplegia

Mean [CI]

70.95 [68.20–73.70]

31.91 [30.88–32.94]

18.35 [17.43–19.27]

4.91 [4.33–5.50]

11.37 [10.32–12.43]

7.18 [6.40–7.96]

9.25 [8.22–10.29]

3.69 [3.59–3.79]

21.06 [20.35–21.78]

Incomplete tetraplegia

Mean [CI]

72.19 [68.60–75.79]

31.16 [29.65–32.66]

18.00 [16.84–19.17]

5.30 [4.50–6.11]

11.88 [10.41–13.35]

6.76 [5.82–7.69]

8.96 [7.69–10.23]

3.68 [3.55–3.82]

20.68 [19.70–21.66]

 

F, df, p

0.94, 3, 0.423

1.86, 3, 0.136

0.40, 3, 0.752

2.80, 3, 0.0400

1.60, 3, 0.188

1.13, 3, 0.338

1.03, 3, 0.381

3.08, 3, 0.027

1.38, 3, 0.247

 

η2

0.006

0.011

0.002

0.016

0.009

0.007

0.006

0.018

0.008

Cause of injury

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Traumatic

Mean [CI]

72.75 [70.98–74.52]

32.37 [31.65–33.09]

17.70 [17.11–18.29]

4.47 [4.09–4.85]

12.38 [11.67–13.08]

6.45 [5.98–6.92]

8.81 [8.14–9.48]

3.80 [3.73–3.87]

21.53 [21.10–21.97]

Non-traumatic

Mean [CI]

71.25 [67.75–74.74]

30.96 [29.55–32.37]

19.06 [17.73–20.39]

5.35 [4.59–6.11]

10.90 [9.40–12.39]

7.89 [6.80–8.98]

9.79 [8.43–11.14]

3.53 [3.40–3.65]

20.16 [19.17–21.16]

 

F, df, p

0.63, 1, 0.428

3.33, 1, 0.069

4.31, 1, 0.039

4.60, 1, 0.033

3.65, 1, 0.057

9.12, 1, 0.003a

1.81, 1, 0.179

14.73, 1, 0.000

2.59, 0.010b

 

η2

0.001

0.006

0.008

0.009

0.007

0.020

0.004

0.028

 

*p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001. aF-test based on log-transformed data. bNon-parametric testing based on unweighted data. Bonferroni corrected alpha level for F-test: p < 0.007 (bold).

CI: 95% confidence interval of the mean value; F: test value; df: degrees of freedom; p: probability value; η2: eta squared effect size; r: Pearson correlation coefficient; MHI-5: Mental Health Index; PANAS-PA: Positive And Negative Affect Schedule – Positive Affect; PANAS-NA: Positive And Negative Affect Schedule – Negative Affect; HADS-D: Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale – Depression subscale; ALE: Appraisal of Life Events Scale – Challenge; ALE Loss: Appraisal of Life Events Scale; 5 WHOQoL: 5 items from the World Health Organisation Quality of Life Scale; PIL-SF: Purpose in Life Test-Short Form.

 

Considering the HADS-D scores, those older at injury and those with a shorter TsI reported slightly more depressed mood, which was also shown in the combined groups.

Thoughts and Beliefs. Considering the evaluation of difficult life situations as a challenge, medium size group differences were detected with respect to age, AaI, TsI and combined groups in the ALE-Challenge (Table IV). Younger participants, participants younger at injury, and those living longer with the injury appraised their situation more in terms of being a challenge. This is also shown in the combined groups, where those older at injury and shorter time since injury reported less challenge evaluations.

In the ALE-Loss subscale small, but significant, differences were found with regards to cause of injury. Participants with non-traumatic, rather than traumatic, SCI tended to make slightly more Loss appraisals.

The ALE subscale Threat did not show significant subgroup differences.

With regards to satisfaction with different life domains, small, but statistically significant, differences in the 5 WHOQoL item scores were detected between the subgroups of AaI, TsI, the combined subgroups, and cause of injury. Those who were younger at injury and those with a longer TsI reported higher life satisfaction. Participants who were older at injury and experienced the injury more recently constitute the least satisfied group. Participants with a traumatic aetiology of injury reported higher life satisfaction than the other lesion groups.

Motives. For the PIL-SF small, but statistically significant, differences were found regarding AaI and the combined AaI and TsI subgroups (Table IV). Participants who were younger at injury report stronger purpose and meaning in life than those older at injury. The combined subgroup of participants who were older at injury, and whose injury happened more recently, reported significantly lower purpose in life than the other subgroups.

Patterns of Experience and Behavior. General self-efficacy measured by the GSES did not differ with regards to sex, age, TsI, combined AaI and TsI subgroups, type of lesion or cause of injury (Table V). A small, but statistically significant, difference was detected regarding AaI. Participants who were younger at injury reported a stronger general self-efficacy than those who were older at injury.

Table V. Subgroup differences in General Self-Efficacy Scale (GSES), Big Five Inventory-21 (BFI-21), and Social Skills Inventory – Short form (SSI-SF) scores in the area Patterns of Experience and Behavior

Population

variable

Statistic

GSES

BFI-21

Openness

BFI-21

Conscientiousness

BFI-21

Extraversion

BFI-21

Agreeableness

BFI-21

Neuroticism

SSI-SF

Expressivity

SSI-SF

Sensitivity

SSI-SF

Control

Sex

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Male

Mean [CI]

30.43 [29.87–30.99]

3.70 [3.63–3.77]

3.79 [3.72–3.86]

3.40 [3.33–3.47]

3.54 [3.47–3.60]

2.45 [2.36–2.53]

30.52 [29.94–31.10]

28.03 [27.46–28.61]

33.81 [33.30–34.32]

Female

Mean [CI]

30.01 [29.02–30.99]

3.87 [3.76–3.98]

3.97 [3.86–4.07]

3.54 [3.40–3.68]

3.74 [3.61–3.87]

2.72 [2.57–2.87]

30.37 [29.31–31.42]

29.88 [28.97–30.80]

32.68 [31.62–33.74]

 

F, df, p

0.61, 1, 0.436

6.47, 1, 0.011

8.37, 1, 0.004

3.75, 1, 0.053

8.62,1, 0.004

11.50, 1, 0.001

0.07,1, 0.729

11.73,1, 0.001

1.75, 0.081a

 

η2

0.001

0.013

0.016

0.007

0.017

0.022

0.000

0.023

 

Age

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Overall

r

–0.10*

0.01

–0.12**

–0.11*

0.09*

0.04

–0.15***

–0.12**

–0.10*

Younger

Mean [CI]

30.88 [30.23–31.53]

3.78 [3.69–3.87]

3.93 [3.85–4.00]

3.52 [3.42–3.62]

3.57 [3.48–3.66]

2.48 [2.37–2.59]

31.29 [30.58–32.00]

29.16 [28.45–29.87]

34.00 [33.30–34.70]

Older

Mean [CI]

29.71 [28.99–30.43]

3.72 [3.64–3.80]

3.76 [3.68–3.84]

3.36 [3.27–3.45]

3.62 [3.54–3.70]

2.58 [2.48–2.67]

29.63 [28.91–30.36]

27.99 [27.32–28.66]

32.94 [32.30–33.58]

 

F, df, p

5.60, 1, 0.018

1.04, 1, 0.309

8.83, 1, 0.003

5.89, 1, 0.016

0.67, 1, 0.414

1.63, 1, 0.202

10.37, 1, 0.001

5.57, 1, 0.019

4.86, 1, 0.028

 

η2

0.011

0.002

0.017

0.011

0.001

0.003

0.020

0.011

0.009

Age at injury

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Overall

r

–0.12**

–0.01

–0.16***

–0.09

0.03

0.08

–0.16***

–0.10*

–0.16***

Younger

Mean [CI]

31.13 [30.51–31.75]

3.76 [3.67–3.85]

3.94 [3.86–4.01]

3.52 [3.43–3.62]

3.60 [3.51–3.69]

2.44 [2.33–2.54]

31.63 [30.91–32.35]

28.98 [28.31–29.65]

34.51 [33.84–35.19]

Older

Mean [CI]

29.58 [28.84–30.31]

3.74 [3.66–3.83]

3.76 [3.68–3.84]

3.37 [3.28–3.46]

3.59 [3.51–3.68]

2.61 [2.50–2.71]

29.45 [28.75–30.16]

28.23 [27.52–28.94]

32.57 [31.91–33.22]

 

F, df, p

2.76, 0.006a

0.08, 1, 0.773

9.70, 1, 0.002

5.21, 1, 0.023

0.01, 1, 0.913

5.20, 1, 0.023

18.11, 1, 0.000

2.24, 1, 0.135

16.60, 1, 0.000

 

η2

 

0.000

0.019

0.010

0.000

0.010

0.034

0.004

0.032

Table V. Contd.

Population

variable

Statistic

GSES

BFI-21

Openness

BFI-21

Conscientiousness

BFI-21

Extraversion

BFI-21

Agreeableness

BFI-21

Neuroticism

SSI-SF

Expressivity

SSI-SF

Sensitivity

SSI-SF

Control

Time since injury

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Overall

r

0.05

0.02

0.09*

–0.01

0.08

–0.07

0.02

–0.01

0.11*

Shorter

Mean [CI]

29.82 [29.10–30.55]

3.72 [3.63–3.80]

3.80 [3.72–3.88]

3.42 [3.33–3.52]

3.56 [3.47–3.65]

2.62 [2.51–2.73]

30.19 [29.47–30.90]

28.59 [27.89–29.28]

32.79 [32.13–33.45]

Longer

Mean [CI]

30.83 [30.19–31.47]

3.79 [3.71–3.88]

3.89 [3.82–3.97]

3.46 [3.37–3.55]

3.63 [3.55–3.72]

2.42 [2.33–2.52]

30.78 [30.05–31.52]

28.57 [27.88–29.26]

34.23 [33.55–34.90]

 

F, df, p

4.13, 1, 0.043

1.55, 1, 0.214

3.07, 1, 0.080

0.36, 1, 0.551

1.16, 1, 0.283

7.05, 1, 0.008

1.32, 1, 0.252

0.00, 1, 0.969

8.95, 1, 0.003

 

η2

0.008

0.003

0.006

0.001

0.002

0.014

0.003

0.000

0.017

Combined

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Younger shorter

Mean [CI]

31.22 [30.02–32.42]

3.66 [3.48–3.84]

3.84 [3.68–4.01]

3.54 [3.32–3.76]

3.44 [3.25–3.63]

2.52 [2.29–2.75]

32.85 [31.43–34.27]

28.90 [27.72–30.09]

34.51 [33.41–35.61]

Younger longer

Mean [CI]

31.10 [30.37–31.82]

3.80 [3.70–3.90]

3.97 [3.88–4.06]

3.52 [3.41–3.62]

3.66 [3.56–3.76]

2.40 [2.29–2.52]

31.19 [30.35–32.02]

29.00 [28.19–29.81]

34.51 [33.69–35.34]

Older shorter

Mean [CI]

29.38 [28.50–30.25]

3.73 [3.64–3.83]

3.78 [3.69–3.87]

3.38 [3.28–3.49]

3.60 ¨3.50–3.70[

2.65 [2.53–2.78]

29.34 [28.55–30.14]

28.49 [27.65–29.33]

32.24 [31.46–33.03]

Older longer

Mean [CI]

30.15 [28.80–31.50]

3.77 [3.60–3.94]

3.70 [3.55–3.85]

3.32 [3.15–3.50]

3.57 [3.40–3.73]

2.47 [2.28–2.66]

29.77 [28.25–31.29]

27.49 [26.16–28.81]

33.51 [32.35–34.67]

 

F, df, p

3.65, 3, 0.013

0.72, 3, 0.541

4.13, 3, 0.007

1.85, 3, 0.137

1.47, 3, 0.223

2.85, 3, 0.037

7.45, 3, 0.000

1.31, 3, 0.272

6.50, 3, 0.000

 

η2

0.021

0.004

0.024

0.011

0.009

0.017

0.042

0.008

0.037

Type of lesion

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Complete Paraplegia

Mean [CI]

30.86 [30.02–31.71]

3.69 [3.59–3.80]

3.86 [3.76–3.95]

3.51 [3.40–3.63]

3.58 [3.47–3.69]

2.45 [2.32–2.58]

31.19 [30.26–32.11]

28.18 [27.43–28.93]

34.32 [33.51–35.12]

Complete tetraplegia

Mean [CI]

29.30 [27.80–30.81]

3.77 [3.59–3.95]

3.78 [3.62–3.94]

3.42 [3.23–3.60]

3.69 [3.52–3.87]

2.52 [2.29–2.74]

31.09 [29.55–32.63]

29.17 [27.64–30.70]

34.20 [33.03–30.70]

Incomplete paraplegia

Mean [CI]

30.24 [29.47–31.01]

3.80 [3.70–3.89]

3.89 [3.80–3.98]

3.37 [3.25–3.48]

3.63 [3.53–3.73]

2.56 [2.43–2.68]

29.97 [29.09–30.86]

28.61 [27.76–29.46]

32.78 [31.94–33.62]

Incomplete tetraplegia

Mean [CI]

30.12 [28.92–31.32]

3.74 [3.60–3.88]

3.76 [3.64–3.89]

3.49 [3.34–3.64]

3.51 [3.37–3.64]

2.58 [2.42–2.75]

30.12 [29.11–31.13]

28.80 [27.61–29.99]

33.27 [32.21–34.33]

 

F, df, p

1.10, 3, 0.349

0.65, 3, 0.580

1.13, 3, 0.337

1.29, 3, 0.277

1.10, 3, 0.350

0.66, 3, 0.578

1.55, 3, 0.201

0.49, 3, 0.686

2.65, 3, 0.049

 

η2

0.006

0.004

0.007

0.008

0.006

0.004

0.009

0.003

0.015

Cause of injury

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Traumatic

Mean [CI]

30.42 [29.89–30.96]

3.73 [3.66–3.80]

3.86 [3.79–3.92]

3.45 [3.38–3.52]

3.57 [3.51–3.64]

2.48 [2.40–2.56]

30.50 [29.95–31.04]

28.29 [27.74–28.84]

33.70 [33.19–34.21]

Non-traumatic

Mean [CI]

29.91 [28.77–31.06]

3.82 [3.69–3.95]

3.80 [3.67–3.93]

3.41 [3.25–3.58]

3.66 [3.52–3.81]

2.69 [2.52–2.87]

30.40 [29.10–31.69]

29.53 [28.47–30.59]

32.74 [31.59–33.90]

 

F, df, p

0.76, 1, 0.384

1.49, 1, 0.222

0.64, 1, 0.423

0.21, 1, 0.644

1.46, 1, 0.228

5.88, 1, 0.016

0.03, 1, 0.872

4.41, 1, 0.036

2.80, 1, 0.095

 

η2

0.001

0.003

0.001

0.000

0.003

0.011

0.000

0.009

0.005

*p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001. aNon-parametric testing based on unweighted data. Bonferroni corrected alpha level for F-test: p < 0.007 (bold).

CI: 95% confidence interval of the mean value; F: test value; df: degrees of freedom; p: probability value; η2: eta-squared effect size; r: Pearson correlation coefficient.

The “big five” personality traits measured by the BFI-21 differed across some examined subgroups. Women scored higher on Conscientiousness, Agreeableness and Neuroticism than males. Conscientiousness was found to be higher in younger age, younger AaI and in the combined younger AaI and longer TsI subgroup.

The indicators of social skills measured by the SSI subscales Expressivity, Sensitivity and Control were found to differ by sex, age, AaI, TsI and the combined subgroups. Younger persons with SCI, those who were younger at injury, and those who were younger and more recently injured reported slightly higher expressivity than the comparison groups. With respect to social Sensitivity, a sex difference was detected with women scoring higher than men. The Control skill was more pronounced in those participants who were younger at injury and with shorter time since injury. Participants who were older at injury and with shorter time since injury reported overall slightly less SSI-Control than the other subgroups.

Among the COPE subscales (Table VI), subgroup differences were not detected for Active coping, Substance use, Behavioral disengagement, Planning, Religion and Self-blame. None of the COPE subscales differed by cause of injury. Self-distraction was used more frequently as a coping strategy by women and those who were more recently injured. Older participants and those who were older at injury reported more frequent use of Denial. Females scored higher in the COPE subscales Emotional support, Instrumental support, and Venting. Younger participants also scored higher in Instrumental support and Venting. Positive reframing was used more frequently by those with longer TsI and with complete paraplegia. The use of Humor as a coping strategy differed with regards to age, AaI as well as the combination of AaI and TsI. Younger persons, persons younger at injury reported more regular use of Humor. Participants who were older at injury and with shorter time since injury scored lowest on the use of Humor and Acceptance as regular coping strategies.

Table VI. Subgroup differences in Brief COPE scores in the area Patterns of Experience and Behavior

 

Statistic

Self

-distraction

Active

coping

Denial

Substance

use

Emotional support

Instrumental support

Behavioral disengagement

Venting

Positive reframing

Planning

Humor

Acceptance

Religion

Self-blame

Sex

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Male

Mean [CI]

4.76 [4.60–4.92]

5.94 [5.79–6.10]

3.28 [3.14–3.43]

2.60 [2.48–2.72]

3.80 (3.65–3.95)

4.17 [4.03–4.32]

3.06 [2.93–3.19]

3.76 [3.62–3.90]

5.62 [5.46–5.78]

5.24 [5.09–5.40]

4.09 [3.93–4.25]

6.45 [6.30–6.60]

3.63 [3.44–3.83]

3.62 [3.48–3.77]

Female

Mean [CI]

5.22 [4.97–5.48]

6.09 [5.84–6.33]

3.15 [2.90–3.41]

2.51 [2.31–2.72]

4.68 [4.40–4.97]

4.89 [4.64–5.13]

3.09 [2.88–3.30]

4.18 [3.93–4.44]

5.54 [5.25–5.82]

5.47 [5.22–5.71]

3.90 [3.59–4.20]

6.33 [6.09–6.56]

3.89 [3.53–4.24]

3.81 [3.52–4.09]

 

F, df, p

9.43, 1, 0.002

0.99, 1, 0.320

0.86, 1, 0.356

0.60, 1, 0.440

32.39, 1, 0.000a

25.67, 1, 0.000

0.06, 1, 0.808

9.50, 1, 0.002

0.27, 1, 0.604

2.41, 1, 0.121

1.47, 1, 0.226

0.73, 1, 0.393

1.76, 1, 0.185

0.47, 1, 0.495a

 

η2

0.018

0.002

0.002

0.001

0.060

0.048

0.000

0.018

0.001

0.005

0.003

0.001

0.003

0.001

Age

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Overall

r

0.03

–0.07

0.12**

–0.08

–0.12**

–0.09*

0.07

–0.18***

–0.05

–0.07

–0.22***

–0.07

0.12**

–0.05

Younger

Mean [CI]

4.83 [4.64–5.02]

6.06 [5.88–6.24]

3.04 [2.88–3.20]

2.63 [2.48–2.78]

4.24 [4.05–4.44]

4.56 [4.38–4.75]

2.97 [2.82–3.12]

4.15 [3.97–4.33]

5.68 [5.48–5.88]

5.41 [5.24–5.59]

4.39 [4.18–4.59]

6.55 [6.38–6.72]

3.59 [3.35.3.83]

3.71 [3.52–3.89]

Older

Mean [CI]

4.97 [4.77–5.16]

5.91 [5.72–6.10]

3.45 [3.26–3.65]

2.52 [2.38–2.66]

3.87 [3.68–4.06]

4.20 [4.02–4.38]

3.17 [3.00–3.33]

3.61 [3.44–3.78]

5.51 [5.31–5.70]

5.20 [5.02–5.39]

3.67 [3.48–3.87]

6.27 [6.08–6.46]

3.83 [3.58–4.07]

3.65 [3.46–3.84]

 

F, df, p

1.01, 1, 0.316

1.37, 1, 0.243

8.31, 1, 0.004a

1.16, 1, 0.283

7.26, 1, 0.007

7.51, 1, 0.006

2.94, 1, 0.087

18.82, 1, 0.000

1.50, 1, 0.221

2.53, 1, 0.113

24.86, 1, 0.000

4.74, 1, 0.030

1.81, 1, 0.179

0.19, 0.662

 

η2

0.002

0.003

0.016

0.002

0.014

0.015

0.006

0.036

0.003

0.005

0.047

0.009

0.004

0.000

Age at injury

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Overall

r

0.08

–0.07

0.16***

–0.06

–0.07

–0.05

0.07

–0.09*

–0.12**

–0.09*

–0.21***

–0.11**

0.11**

–0.02

Younger

Mean [CI]

4.75 [4.56–4.93]

6.05 [5.88–6.21]

3.01 [2.86–3.17]

2.67 [2.51–2.83]

4.12 [3.92–4.31]

4.44 [4.26–4.62]

3.01 [2.87–3.16]

3.97 [3.78–4.15]

5.79 [5.61–5.97]

5.37 [5.20–5.54]

4.36 [4.15–4.56]

6.57 [6.40–6.74]

3.57 [3.33–3.81]

3.68 [3.50–3.86]

Older

Mean [CI]

5.03 [4.83–5.23]

5.93 [5.74–6.13]

3.45 [3.25–3.64]

2.49 [2.36–2.63]

4.01 [3.81–4.20]

4.33 [4.15–4.52]

3.11 [2.95–3.28]

3.81 [3.64–3.98]

5.42 [5.22–5.63]

5.25 [5.06–5.45]

3.75 [3.55–3.95]

6.27 [6.09–6.46]

3.83 [3.59–4.07]

3.68 [3.49–3.86]

 

F, df, p

4.35, 1, 0.038

0.57, 0.57b

–3.01, 0.003b

1.81, 0.071b

0.60, 1, 0.439

0.66, 1, 0.418

0.75, 1, 0.386

1.55, 1, 0.214

2.24, 0.025b

0.84, 1, 0.360

17,35, 1, 0.000

5.42,1, 0.020

2.25, 1, 0.134

0.00, 1, 0.981

 

η2

0.008

 

 

 

0.001

0.001

0.001

0.003

 

0.002

0.033

0.011

0.004

0.000

Time since injury

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Overall

r

–0.08

0.01

–0.09

0.00

–0.03

–0.04

–0.01

–0.09*

0.11*

0.03

0.03

0.08

–0.01

–0.03

Shorter

Mean [CI]

5.08 [4.89–5.27]

6.03 [5.84–6.21]

3.39 [3.20–3.58]

2.56 [2.42–2.70]

4.10 [3.90–4.30]

4.44 [4.26–4.62]

3.06 [2.90–3.22]

4.02 [3.84–4.20]

5.38 [5.17–5.58]

5.28 [5.10–5.46]

3.91 [3.70–4.11]

6.26 [6.07–6.45]

3.73 [3.49–3.97]

3.72 [3.52–3.92]

Longer

Mean [CI]

4.70 [4.51–4.89]

5.94 [5.76–6.13]

3.08 [2.91–3.25]

2.59 [2.44–2.74]

4.01 [3.82–4.21]

4.33 [4.14–4.51]

3.08 [2.92–3.24]

3.74 [3.57–3.91]

5.83 [5.65–6.01]

5.34 [5.16–5.52]

4.17 [3.97–4.38]

6.58 [6.41–6.75]

3.68 [3.44–3.93]

3.64 [3.46–3.81]

 

F, df, p

7.82, 1, 0.005

0.38, 1, 0.537

5.78, 1, 0.017

0.06, 1, 0.806

0.37, 1, 0.541

0.74, 1, 0.389

0.04, 1, 0.844

4.82, 1, 0.029

10.41, 1,0.001

0.19.1, 0.660

3.31, 1, 0.069

6.35, 1, 0.012

0.07, 1, 0.794

0.41, 1, 0.523

 

η2

0.015

0.001

0.011

0.000

0.001

0.001

0.000

0.009

0.020

0.000

0.006

0.012

0.000

0.001

Combined

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Younger shorter

Mean [CI]

4.87 [4.49–5.25]

6.14 [5.81–6.47]

3.10 [2.78–3,42]

2.70 [2.39–3.01]

4.36 [3.92–4.80]

4.41 [4.05–4.76]

2.93 [2.62–3.25]

4.27 [3.85–4.70]

5.58 [5.19–5.98]

5.39 [5.07–5.72]

4.64 [4.20–5.07]

6.67 [6.35–6.99]

3.42 [2.97–3.87]

3.91 [3.47–4.36]

Younger longer

Mean [CI]

4.70 [4.49–4.91]

6.01 [5.82–6.21]

2.98 [2.80–3.16]

2.66 [2.48–2.85]

4.03 [3.81–4.24]

4.45 [4.24–4.67]

3.04 [2.87–3.21]

3.86 [3.66–4.06]

5.86 [5.66–6.06]

5.37 [5.17–5.57]

4.25 [4.03–4.48]

6.53 [6.34–6.73]

3.62 [3.33–3.91]

3.60 [3.40–3.79]

Older shorter

Mean [CI]

5.15 [4.93–5.37]

5.99 [5.77–6.21]

3.49 [3.26–3.71]

2.52 [2.36–2.68]

4.02 [3.79–4.24]

4.45 [4.24–4.66]

3.09 [2.91–3.28]

3.94 [3.74–4.13]

5.31 [5.07–5.56]

5.25 [5.03–5.46]

3.68 [3.45–3.90]

6.13 [5.91–6.35]

3.83 [3.54–4.11]

3.66 [3.44–3.88]

Older longer

Mean [CI]

4.70 [4.26–5.13]

5.78 [5.33–6.22]

3.34 [2.95–3.73]

2.40 [2.15–2.66]

3.98 [3.55–4.40]

4.00 [3.62–4.39]

3.17 [2.80–3.53]

3.45 [3.13–3.76]

5.75 [5.34–6.16]

5.27 [4.85–5.70]

3.97 [3.55–4.40]

6.69 [6.38–7.00]

3.84 [3.37–4.31]

3.73 [3.37–4.10]

 

F, df, p

3.12,3, 0.026

0.74, 3, 0.864b

3.59, 3, 0.014a

1.16, 3, 0.323

0.88, 3, 0.452

1.81, 3, 0.145

0.42, 3, 0.742

3.92, 3, 0.009

9.08, 3, 0.028b

0.29, 3, 0.833

7.25, 3, 0.000

4.63, 3, 0.003

0.91, 3, 0.437

0.74, 3, 0.531

 

η2

0.018

 

0.021

0.007

0.005

0.011

0.002

0.023

 

0.002

0.041

0.027

0.005

0.004

Table VI. Contd.

 

Statistic

Self

-distraction

Active

coping

Denial

Substance

use

Emotional support

Instrumental support

Behavioral disengagement

Venting

Positive reframing

Planning

Humor

Acceptance

Religion

Self-blame

Type of lesion

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Complete paraplegia

Mean [CI]

4.79 [4.54–5.04]

6.15 [5.92–6.37]

3.13 [2.92–3.34]

2.55 [2.36–2.73]

4.15 [3.90–4.39]

4.41 [4.19–4.64]

3.01 [2.82–3.21]

3.85 [3.65 [4.06]

5.98 [5.76–6.21]

5.48 [5.26–5.69]

4.49 [4.21–4.76]

6.59 [6.38–6.80]

3.45 [3.15–3.74]

3.55 [3.32–3.77]

Complete tetraplegia

Mean [CI]

5.04 [4.67–5.42]

5.61 [5.20–6.03]

2.83 [2.50–3.15]

2.66 [2.37–2.96]

3.90 [3.52–4.27]

4.05 [3.72–4.38]

2.96 [2.65–3.28]

4.17 [3.75–4.59]

5.42 [5.03–5.81]

5.43 [5.06–5.80]

3.91 [3.50–4.32]

6.53 [6.16–6.91]

3.53 [3.00–4.06]

3.76 [3.40–4.11]

Incomplete para

Mean [CI]

4.94 [4.72–5.17]

5.98 [5.77–6.18]

3.38 [3.16–3.60]

2.60 [2.41–2.78]

3.99 [3.76–4.22]

4.31 [4.09–4.52]

3.10 [2.90–3.30]

3.80 [3.59–4.00]

5.44 [5.20–5.68]

5.18 [4.97–5.39]

3.81 [3.59–4.03]

6.26 [6.03–6.48]

4.00 [3.70–4.29]

3.75 [3.52–3.98]

Íncomplete tetra

Mean [CI]

4.90 [4.58–5.21]

5.96 [5.64–6.28]

3.35 [3.05–3.65]

2.53 [2.33–2.74]

4.14 [3.81–4.46]

4.64 [4.34–4.95]

3.13 [2.89–3.37]

3.96 [3.68–4.24]

5.42 [5.09–5.75]

5.27 [4.95–5.59]

3.88 [3.56–4.20]

6.40 [6.13–6.67]

3.61 [3.26–3.97]

3.70 [3.40–3.99]

 

F, df, p

0.45, 3, 0.721

1.67, 3, 0.172

2.47, 3, 0.061

0.19, 3, 0.905

0.53, 3, 0.660

2.18, 3, 0.090

0.33, 3, 0.806

1.05, 3, 0.368

4.27, 3, 0.005

1.31, 3, 0.269

5.50, 3, 0.001

1.68, 3, 0.171

2.58, 3, 0.053

0.58, 3, 0.628

 

η2

0.003

0.010

0.014

0.001

0.003

0.013

0.002

0.006

0.025

0.008

0.032

0.010

0.015

0.003

Cause of injury

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Traumatic

Mean [CI]

4.84 [4.69–4.98]

6.02 [5.87–6.16]

3.16 [3.02–3.30]

2.55 [2.44–2.66]

3.96 [3.80–4.11]

4.31 [4.16–4.45]

2.99 [2.86–3.11]

3.81 [3.67–3.95]

5.61 [5.46–5.77]

5.31 [5.16–5.45]

4.12 [3.96–4.28]

6.50 [6.36–6.63]

3.71 [3.51–3.90]

3.73 [3.59–3.88]

Non-traumatic

Mean [CI]

5.11 [4.79–5.43]

5.89 [5.62–6.17]

3.51 [3.21–3.81]

2.67 [2.41–2.93]

4.40 [4.07–4.73]

4.64 [4.35–4.93]

3.34 [3.08–3.59]

4.12 [3.85–4.38]

5.53 [5.21–5.85]

5.32 [5.06–5.59]

3.75 [3.43–4.06]

6.14 [5.84–6.44]

3.71 [3.33–4.08]

3.51 [3.20–3.81]

 

F, df, p

2.74, 1, 0.098

0.60, 1, 0.440

5.21, 1, 0.023

0.93, 1, 0.335

7.16, 1, 0.008

4.53, 1, 0.034

6.86, 1, 0.009

4.09, 1, 0.044

0.26, 1, 0.613

0.02, 1, 0.900

4.73, 1, 0.030

5.69, 1, 0.018

0.00, 1, 0.996

2.01, 1, 0.156

 

η2

0.005

0.001

0.010

0.002

0.014

0.009

0.0113

0.008

0.001

0.000

0.009

0.011

0.000

0.004

*p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001. aF-test based on log-transformed data. bNon-parametric testing based on unweighted data.

CI: 95% confidence interval of the mean value; F: test value; df: degrees of freedom; p: probability value; η2: eta squared effect size; r: Pearson correlation coefficient. Bonferroni corrected alpha level for F-test: p < 0.007 (bold).

 

DISCUSSION

In the present study, functioning and health of individuals with SCI is described in the light of PPFs. The PPF areas of Feelings, Thoughts, Motives, and Patterns of Experience and Behavior were assessed using a set of measurement instruments selected according to a predefined rationale in line with the ICF (53).

The results show that most individuals in the present SwiSCI sample experienced little negative affect and depressed feelings, but moderately strong positive affect in the week before the data collection. The majority of participants did not evaluate difficult life situations as loss or threat, but to some degree as a positive challenge. Participants were rather satisfied with life and experienced a strong sense of purpose in life. They reported moderately strong self-efficacy, scored low on the personality trait of neuroticism and in the middle range of social skills. They used problem-oriented coping strategies (15) (e.g. active coping, positive reframing, planning) with moderate to high frequency and less frequently adopt dysfunctional avoidance-oriented coping (e.g. substance use or behavioural disengagement) (22).

The analyses show that statistically significant differences between age at injury and time since injury subgroups can be detected across different PPF areas. Participants who were older (> 32 years) and sustained their injury more recently (< 14 years) experienced more depressed mood, less positive affect and challenge appraisals, lower life satisfaction, purpose in life and self-efficacy. They reported lower social skills, less usage of the coping strategies humour, positive reframing, and acceptance, but more usage of denial and self-distraction strategies.

Sex differences and differences by chronological age were more frequently detected in the PPF area of Patterns of Experience and Behavior than the other PPF areas. Male participants reported less conscientiousness compared with females, less neuroticism, less social sensitivity, and less regular usage of the coping strategies self-distraction, emotional support, instrumental support, and venting. The sex differences in the emotional and support-focused coping strategies (emotional support, instrumental support, venting) observed in the SwiSCI sample largely correspond with findings from other populations (92).

Persons with non-traumatic injuries were less satisfied with life and experienced more loss appraisals than participants with traumatic injuries.

However, subgroup differences were overall small in effect size. The strongest differences were detected for the cognitive appraisal of stressful situations as a challenge. Here, age, age at injury, and the combined subgroups of age at, and time since, injury differed with a medium effect size.

Although the current sample seems to be well adjusted to SCI in showing little negative affect and depressive feelings, those with higher age at injury and shorter time since injury show overall more negative feelings and mood, thoughts, beliefs, motives and patterns of experience and behaviour.

Previous research indicates that older adults with physical disabilities are more likely to report symptoms of depression than younger persons (93). Negative feelings and mood may increase with higher age, when medical complications and dependence on others for assistance increase (94) while a loss of social roles occurs (95). Younger individuals may have a longer time perspective, may have more strategies to cope with the consequences of SCI, and are therefore more likely to go about reaching their life goals (96, 97).

With regards to time since injury, the comparison with previous studies shows mixed results in different PPF areas. For example, in the SwiSCI sample time since injury was unrelated to general self-efficacy, which is in line with previous findings (17). In the current sample, longer time since injury was associated with less depressed feelings and more positive affect, in line with some previous findings on depression, anxiety and distress (98–100), while in other studies level of depressive symptoms increased with longer time since injury (101, 102). Furthermore, in this study, longer time since injury was associated with higher satisfaction with quality of life similar to some earlier research (103, 104), while other studies did not find significant relationships between time since injury and quality of life (105–108). In particular, longitudinal studies indicate that life satisfaction may not necessarily change (94), but that individuals with SCI maintain relatively good and stable life satisfaction over time (102, 109). Research examining trajectories in mental health after SCI revealed a group of individuals with chronic low mental health, a highly resilient group, a group showing early and a group showing delayed recovery from SCI (12, 110).

Overall, the present findings hint at a certain relevance of time-dependent processes, which seem to be yet insufficiently understood in relation to PPFs in persons with SCI and warrants further research.

The current findings must be interpreted with caution, in particular due to limitations regarding the generalizability of the results. Considering the overall response rate of SwiSCI, many individuals with SCI who were potentially eligible for study participation chose not to take part in the data collection. It can be assumed that persons with more negative experience across PPF areas (e.g. higher depression, lower life satisfaction, or higher substance use) and individuals with severe physical and mental health problems are less likely to participate in such type of surveys and thus may be underrepresented in the present sample (111). Therefore, the results may underestimate negative experience of persons with SCI in Switzerland, even though some adjustment for non-response bias has been undertaken in the analyses.

The use of median-splits in the analyses is a further critical point, which can lead to loss of power and loss of information related to the distribution of data. In addition, the generalizability of results can be limited, since the comparison groups are created based on the current sample only. However, median-splits are used in exploratory research in order to create groups of equal size for further analyses, such as ANOVA, and to simplify analyses and reporting in large data-sets. Considering the exploratory nature of the analyses and the small effect sizes, the results for subgroup differences must be regarded as preliminary and require further testing.

Future research with longitudinal study designs is required in order to improve our understanding of time-dependent processes, especially in relation to time since injury, and differentiate between the effects of age and aging, birth cohort, and adjustment processes. Future research also needs to account for complex interrelationships of PPFs and to consider the potential non-linearity of the statistical associations over time. Comparative research between SCI and the general population regarding distributions of PPFs is necessary for the further understanding of functioning and health of persons with SCI. In addition, further conceptual work is required to clarify the potential relationships of PPFs with adjustment, its components and determinants.

Regarding clinical practice, the comparatively more negative experience in the subgroup of older and more recently injured persons suggests a need for specifically tailored and carefully timed psychotherapeutic interventions for this group in order to enhance a more positive emotional experience, more functional cognitions and beliefs, stronger awareness of own motives and acquisition of more adaptive coping strategies.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

The authors would like to thank Martin Brinkhof, Birgit Prodinger and Carolina Ballert for their methodological advice and support. The authors gratefully acknowledge the contribution of all SwiSCI participants.

This study was financed within the framework of the Swiss Spinal Cord Injury Cohort Study (SwiSCI, www.swisci.ch), supported by the Swiss Paraplegic Research. The members of the SwiSCI Steering Committee are: Olivier Deriaz (Clinique Romande de Readaptation, Sion); Michael Baumberger and Hans Peter Gmünder (Swiss Paraplegic Center, Nottwil); Armin Curt and Martin Schubert (University Clinic Balgrist, Zürich); Kerstin Hug and Margret Hund-Georgiadis (REHAB Basel, Basel); Hans Georg Koch and Urs Styger (Swiss Paraplegic Association, Nottwil); Hardy Landolt (representative for persons with SCI, Glarus); Rita Schaumann-Von Stosch (SUVA, Luzern); Mirjam Brach and Gerold Stucki (Swiss Paraplegic Research, Nottwil); and Martin Brinkhof and Christine Thyrian (SwiSCI Study Center at Swiss Paraplegic Research, Nottwil).

REFERENCES

1. Craig A, Tran Y. Psychological aspects associated with spinal cord injury rehabilitation: new directions and best evidence. New York: Nova Publications; 2008.

2. Jensen MP, Truitt AR, Schomer KG, Yorkston KM, Baylor C, Molton IR. Frequency and age effects of secondary health conditions in individuals with spinal cord injury: a scoping review. Spinal Cord 2013; 51: 882–892.

3. Galvin LR, Godfrey HP. The impact of coping on emotional adjustment to spinal cord injury (SCI): review of the literature and application of a stress appraisal and coping formulation. Spinal Cord 2001; 39: 615–627.

4. Orenczuk S, Slivinski J, Mehta S, Teasell RW. Depression following spinal cord injury. Spinal Cord Injury Rehabilitation Evidence (SCIRE). Version 3.0. 2010 [Accessed 2015 Jul 2]. Available from: http://www.scireproject.com/.

5. Krause JS, Saunders LL, Newman S. Posttraumatic stress disorder and spinal cord injury. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 2010; 91: 1182–1187.

6. Biering-Sorensen F, Scheuringer M, Baumberger M, Charlifue SW, Post MW, Montero F, et al. Developing core sets for persons with spinal cord injuries based on the International Classification of Functioning, Disability and Health as a way to specify functioning. Spinal Cord 2006; 44: 541–546.

7. Cieza A, Kirchberger I, Biering-Sorensen F, Baumberger M, Charlifue S, Post MW, et al. ICF Core Sets for individuals with spinal cord injury in the long-term context. Spinal Cord 2010; 48: 305–312.

8. Kirchberger I, Cieza A, Biering-Sorensen F, Baumberger M, Charlifue S, Post MW, et al. ICF Core Sets for individuals with spinal cord injury in the early post-acute context. Spinal Cord 2010; 48: 297–304.

9. Bombardier CH, Richards JS, Krause JS, Tulsky D, Tate DG. Symptoms of major depression in people with spinal cord injury: implications for screening. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 2004; 85: 1749–1756.

10. Schonherr MC, Groothoff JW, Mulder GA, Schoppen T, Eisma WH. Vocational reintegration following spinal cord injury: expectations, participation and interventions. Spinal Cord 2004; 42: 177–184.

11. Dijkers MP. Quality of life of individuals with spinal cord injury: a review of conceptualization, measurement, and research findings. J Rehabil Res Dev 2005; 42: 87–110.

12. Van Leeuwen CM, Post MW, Hoekstra T, van der Woude LH, de Groot S, Snoek GJ, et al. Trajectories in the course of life satisfaction after spinal cord injury: identification and predictors. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 2011; 92: 207–213.

13. van Koppenhagen CF, Post MW, van der Woude LH, de Witte LP, van Asbeck FW, de Groot S, et al. Changes and determinants of life satisfaction after spinal cord injury: a cohort study in the Netherlands. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 2008; 89: 1733–1740.

14. Middleton J, Craig A. Psychological challenges in treating persons with spinal cord injury. In: Craig A, Tran Y, editors. Psychological aspects associated with spinal cord injury rehabilitation: new directions and best evidence. New York: Nova Science Publishers, Inc.; 2008.

15. Livneh H, Martz E. Adjustment to chronic illness and disability: theoretical perspectives, empirical findings, and unresolved issues. In: Kennedy P, editor. The Oxford handbook of rehabilitation psychology. New York: Oxford University Press; 2012.

16. Chevalier Z, Kennedy P, Sherlock O. Spinal cord injury, coping and psychological adjustment: a literature review. Spinal Cord 2009; 47: 778–782.

17. Peter C, Muller R, Cieza A, Geyh S. Psychological resources in spinal cord injury: a systematic literature review. Spinal Cord 2012; 50: 188–201.

18. Muller R, Peter C, Cieza A, Geyh S. The role of social support and social skills in people with spinal cord injury-a systematic review of the literature. Spinal Cord 2012; 50: 94–106.

19. Kennedy P, Evans M, Sandhu N. Psychological adjustment to spinal cord injury: the contribution of coping, hope and cognitive appraisals. Psychol Health Med 2009; 14: 17–33.

20. Kennedy P, Lude P, Elfstrom ML, Smithson E. Sense of coherence and psychological outcomes in people with spinal cord injury: appraisals and behavioural responses. Br J Health Psychol 2010; 15: 611–621.

21. Kennedy P, Lude P, Elfstrom ML, Smithson E. Cognitive appraisals, coping and quality of life outcomes: a multi-centre study of spinal cord injury rehabilitation. Spinal Cord 2010; 48: 762–769.

22. van Leeuwen CM, Kraaijeveld S, Lindeman E, Post MW. Associations between psychological factors and quality of life ratings in persons with spinal cord injury: a systematic review. Spinal Cord 2012; 50: 174–187.

23. Hampton NZ. Self-efficacy and quality of life in people with spinal cord injuries in China. Rehabil Counsel Bull 2000; 43: 66–74.

24. Hampton NZ. Subjective Well-being among people with spinal cord injuries: the role of self-efficacy, perceived social support, and perceived health. Rehabil Counsel Bull 2004; 48: 31–37.

25. Hampton NZ. The affective aspect of subjective well-being among Chinese people with and without spinal cord injuries. Disabil Rehabil 2008; 30: 1473–1479.

26. Hampton NZ, Marshall A. Culture, gender, self-efficacy, and life satisfaction: a comparison between Americans and Chinese people with spinal cord injuries. J Rehabil 2000; 66: 21–28.

27. de Roon-Cassini TA, de St Aubin E, Valvano A, Hastings J, Horn P. Psychological well-being after spinal cord injury: perception of loss and meaning making. Rehabil Psychol 2009; 54: 306–314.

28. White B, Driver S, Warren AM. Resilience and indicators of adjustment during rehabilitation from a spinal cord injury. Rehabil Psychol 2010; 55: 23–32.

29. Mona LR, Krause JS, Norris FH, Cameron RP, Kalichman SC, Lesondak LM. Sexual expression following spinal cord injury. Neurorehabilitation 2000; 15: 121–131.

30. Chevalier Z, Kennedy P, Sherlock O. Spinal cord injury, coping and psychological adjustment: a literature review. Spinal Cord 2009; 47: 778–782

31. Song HY. Modeling social reintegration in persons with spinal cord injury. Disabil Rehabil 2005; 27: 131–141.

32. Anderson CJ, Vogel LC, Chlan KM, Betz RR. Coping with spinal cord injury: strategies used by adults who sustained their injuries as children or adolescents. J Spinal Cord Med 2008; 31: 290–296.

33. Shanmugham K, Elliott TR, Palmatier A. Social problem solving abilities and psychosocial impairment among individuals recuperating from surgical repair for severe pressure sores. NeuroRehabilitation 2004; 19: 259–269.

34. Elliott T, Godshall FJ, Herrick SM, Witty TE. Problem-solving appraisal and psychological adjustment following spinal cord injury. Cognit Ther Res 1991; 15: 387–398.

35. Elliott T. Social problem-solving abilities and adjustment to recent-onset spinal cord injury. Rehabil Psychol 1999; 44: 315–332.

36. Berry J, Elliott TR, Rivera P. Resilient, undercontrolled, and overcontrolled personality prototypes among persons with spinal cord injury. J Pers Assess 2007; 89: 292–302.

37. Kennedy P, Taylor N, Hindson L. A pilot investigation of a psychosocial activity course for people with spinal cord injuries. Psychol Health Med 2006; 11: 91–99.

38. Nicholson Perry K, Nicholas MK, Middleton J, Siddall P. Psychological characteristics of people with spinal cord injury-related persisting pain referred to a tertiary pain management center. J Rehabil Res Dev 2009; 46: 57–67.

39. Pang MY, Eng JJ, Lin KH, Tang PF, Hung C, Wang YH. Association of depression and pain interference with disease-management self-efficacy in community-dwelling individuals with spinal cord injury. J Rehabil Med 2009; 41: 1068–1073.

40. Nicholson Perry K, Nicholas MK, Middleton J. Spinal cord injury-related pain in rehabilitation: a cross-sectional study of relationships with cognitions, mood and physical function. Eur J Pain 2009; 13: 511–517.

41. Shnek ZM, Foley FW, LaRocca NG, Gordon WA, DeLuca J, Schwartzman HG, et al. Helplessness, self-efficacy, cognitive distortions, and depression in multiple sclerosis and spinal cord injury. Ann Behav Med 1997; 19: 287–294.

42. Spungen MI, Libin A, Ljungberg I, Groah S. Self-efficacy mediating the occurrence of secondary conditions after spinal cord injury. SCI Psychosocial Process 2009; 22: 16–22.

43. Coyle CP, Lesnik-Emas S, Kinney WB. Predicting life satisfaction among adults with spinal cord injuries. Rehabil Psychol 1994; 39: 95–112.

44. Gorman C, Kennedy P, Hamilton LR. Alterations in self-perceptions following childhood onset of spinal cord injury. Spinal Cord 1998; 36: 181–185.

45. Berry JW, Elliott TR, Rivera P. Resilient, undercontrolled, and overcontrolled personality prototypes among persons with spinal cord injury. J Pers Assess 2007; 89: 292–302.

46. Lee Y. Does self-monitoring influence the experience of leisure for individuals with spinal cord injury? Ann Therapeut Recreat 2008; 16: 39–48.

47. Molton IR, Jensen MP, Nielson W, Cardenas D, Ehde DM. A preliminary evaluation of the motivational model of pain self-management in persons with spinal cord injury-related pain. J Pain 2008; 9: 606–612.

48. Hampton NZ. Subjective well-being among people with spinal cord injuries: the role of self-efficacy, perceived social support, and perceived health. Rehabil Couns Bull 2004; 48: 31–37.

49. Kennedy P, Lude P, Taylor N. Quality of life, social participation, appraisals and coping post spinal cord injury: a review of four community samples. Spinal Cord 2006; 44: 95–105.

50. Van Leeuwen CMC, Post M, Westers P, van der Woude LHV, de Groot S, Sluis T, et al. Relationships between activities, participation, personal factors, mental health, and life satisfaction in persons with spinal cord injury. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 2012; 93: 82–89.

51. Hartoonian N, Hoffman JM, Kalpakjian CZ, Taylor HB, Krause JK, Bombardier CH. Evaluating a spinal cord injury-specific model of depression and quality of life. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 2014; 95: 455–465.

52. World Health Organization. International Classification of Functioning, Disability and Health: ICF. Geneva: World Health Organization; 2001.

53. Geyh S, Müller R, Peter C, Bickenbach J, Post MWM, Stucki G, et al. Capturing the psychologic-personal perspective in spinal cord injury. Am J Phys Med Rehabil 2011; 90: S79–S96.

54. Post MW, Brinkhof M, von Elm E, Boldt C, Brach M, Muff C, et al. Design of the Swiss Spinal Cord Injury Cohort Study. Am J Phys Med Rehab 2011; 90: S5–S16.

55. Prodinger B, Ballert CS., Cieza A. Setting up a cohort study of functioning: From classification to measurement. J Rehabil Med 2016, 48: 131–140.

56. Brinkhof MWG, Fekete C, Chamberlain JD, Post MWM, Gemperli A. Swiss national community survey on functioning after spinal cord injury: protocol, characteristics of participants and determinants of nonresponse. J Rehabil Med 2016, 48: 120–130.

57. World Health Organization. Process of translation and adaptation of instruments. World Health Organization; 2014 [cited 2014]; Available from: http: //www.who.int/substance_abuse/research_tools/translation/en/.

58. Ware JE, Snow KK, Kosinski M, Gandek B. SF-36 health survey: manual and interpretation guide. University of Michigan: The Health Institute, New England Medical Center; 1993.

59. Friedman B, Heisel M, Delavan R. Validity of the SF-36 five-item Mental Health Index for major depression in functionally impaired, community-dwelling elderly patients. J Am Geriatr Soc 2005; 53: 1978–1985.

60. Watson D, Clark LA, Tellegen A. Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. J Pers Soc Psychol 1988; 54: 1063–1070.

61. Zevon MA, Tellegen A. The structure of mood change: an idiographic/nomothetic analysis. J Pers Soc Psychol 1982; 43: 111–122.

62. Ostir GV, Smith PM, Smith D, Ottenbacher KJ. Reliability of the positive and negative affect schedule (PANAS) in medical rehabilitation. Clin Rehabil 2005; 19: 767–769.

63. Zigmond AS, Snaith RP. The Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale. Acta Psychiatr Scand 1983; 67: 361–370.

64. Muller R, Cieza A, Geyh S. Rasch analysis of the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale in spinal cord injury. Rehabil Psychol 2012; 57: 214–223.

65. Snaith R, Zigmond A. The Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale Manual. Nelson, Windsor: NFER; 1994.

66. Ferguson E, Matthews G, Cox T. The Appraisal of Life Events (ALE) scale: reliability and validity. Br J Health Psychol 1999; 4: 97–116.

67. Kennedy P, Lude P, Elfström ML, Smithson E. Sense of coherence and psychological quality of life in people with spinal cord injury: appraisals and behavioural responses. Br J Health Psychol 2010; 15: 611–621.

68. Kennedy P, Lude P, Elfstrom ML, Smithson E. Cognitive appraisals, coping and quality of life outcomes: a multi-centre study of spinal cord injury rehabilitation. Spinal Cord 2010; 48: 762–769.

69. Gourounti K, Anagnostopoulos F, Vaslamatzis G. Primary appraisal of infertility: Evaluation of the psychometric properties of a Greek version of the Appraisal of Life Events Scale (ALE) in a sample of infertile women undergoing fertility treatment. Women Health 2010; 50: 688–704.

70. Geyh S, Fellinghauer BAG, Kirchberger I, Post MWM. Cross-cultural validity of four quality of life scales in persons with spinal cord injury. Health Qual Life Outcomes 2010; 8: 1–16.

71. WHOQOL Group. Development of the World Health Organization WHOQOL-BREF quality of life assessment. Psychol Med 1998; 28: 551–558.

72. Lin MR, Hwang HF, Chen CY, Chiu WT. Comparisons of the brief form of the World Health Organization Quality of Life and Short Form-36 for persons with spinal cord injuries. Am J Phys Med Rehabil 2007; 86: 104–113.

73. Schulenberg SE, Schnetzer LW, Buchanan EM. The purpose in life test-short form: development and psychometric support. J Happiness Stud 2010; 12: 861–876.

74. Crumbaugh JC, Maholick LT. An experimental study in existentialism: The psychometric approach to Frankl’s concept of noogenic neurosis. J Clin Psychol 1964; 20: 200–207.

75. Schwarzer R, Bäßler J, Kwiatek P, Schröder K. The assessment of optimistic self-beliefs: Comparison of the German, Spanish, and Chinese versions of the General Self-efficacy Scale. Appl Psychol 1997; 46: 69–88.

76. Scholz U, Doña BG, Sud S, Schwarzer R. Is general self-efficacy a universal construct? Psychometric findings from 25 countries. Eur J Psychol Assess 2002; 18: 242–251.

77. Schwarzer R. Measurement of perceived self-eficacy. Psychometric scales for crosscultural research. Berlin: Freie Universitat Berlin; 1993.

78. Schwarzer R, Bäßler J, Kwiatek P, Schröder K. The assessment of optimistic self-beliefs: Comparison of the German, Spanish, and Chinese versions of the General Self-efficacy Scale. Appl Psychol-Int Rev 1997; 46: 69–88.

79. Rammstedt B, John O. Kurzversion des Big Five Inventory (BFI-K): Entwicklung und Validierung eines ökonomischen Inventars zur Erfassung der fünf Faktoren der Persönlichkeit. Diagnostica 2005; 51: 195–206.

80. Riggio RE, Canary DR. Social skills inventory manual (2nd edition). Redwood City, CA: MindGarden; 2003.

81. Riggio RE. Assessment of basic socail skills. J Pers Soc Psychol 1986; 51: 649–660.

82. Riggio R. The social skills inventory (SSI): measuring nonverbal and social skills. In: Manusov V, editor. The sourcebook of nonverbal measures: going beyond words. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers; 2005.

83. Carver CS. You want to measure coping but your protocol’s too long: consider the Brief COPE. Int J Behav Med 1997; 4: 92–100.

84. Carver CS, Scheier MF, Weintraub JK. Assessing coping strategies: a theoretically based approach. J Pers Soc Psychol 1989; 56: 267–283.

85. Kennedy P, Marsh N, Lowe R, Grey N, Short E, Rogers B. A longitudinal analysis of psychological impact and coping strategies following spinal cord injury. Br J Health Psychol 2000; 5: 157–172.

86. Kapsou M, Panayiotou G, Kokkinos CM, Demetriou AG. Dimensionality of coping: An empirical contribution to the construct validation of the Brief-COPE with a Greek-speaking sample. J Health Psychol 2010; 15: 215–229.

87. DeCarlo LT. On the meaning and use of kurtosis. Psychol Methods 1997; 2: 292–307.

88. Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences, 2nd edition. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1988.

89. Schmider E, Ziegler M, Danay E, Beyer L, Bühner M. Is it really robust? Reinvestigating the robustness of ANOVA against violations of the normal distribution assumption. Methodology 2010; 6: 147–151.

90. StataCorp. Stata Statistical Software: Release 13. College Station, TX: StataCorp LP; 2013.

91.R. missForest: Nonparametric Missing Value Imputation using Random Forest. 2013 [cited 2014]; Available from: http: //cran.r-project.org/web/packages/missForest/index.html.

92. Tamres LK, Janicki D, Helgeson VS. Sex differences in coping behavior: a meta-analytic review and an examination of relative coping. Pers Soc Psychol Rev 2002; 6: 2–30.

93. Bombardier CH, Ehde DM, Stoelb B, Molton IR. The relationship of age-related factors to psychological functioning among people with disabilities. Phys Med Rehabil Clin N Am 2010; 21: 281–297.

94. Charlifue S, Jha A, Lammertse D. Aging with spinal cord injury. Phys Med Rehabil Clin N Am 2010; 21: 383–402.

95. Krause JS. Aging and life adjustment after spinal cord injury. Spinal Cord 1998; 36: 320–328.

96. Bellizzi KM. Expressions of generativity and posttraumatic growth in adult cancer survivors. Int J Aging Hum Dev 2004; 58: 267–287.

97. Evers AW, Kraaimaat FW, van Lankveld W, Jongen PJ, Jacobs JW, Bijlsma JW. Beyond unfavorable thinking: the illness cognition questionnaire for chronic diseases. J Consult Clin Psychol 2001; 69: 1026–1036.

98. Migliorini C, Tonge B, Taleporos G. Spinal cord injury and mental health. Aust N Z J Psychiatry 2008; 42: 309–314.

99. Saunders LL, Krause JS, Focht KL. A longitudinal study of depression in survivors of spinal cord injury. Spinal Cord 2012; 50: 72–77.

100. Schonenberg M, Reimitz M, Jusyte A, Maier D, Badke A, Hautzinger M. Depression, posttraumatic stress, and risk factors following spinal cord injury. Int J Behav Med 2014; 21: 169–176.

101. Krause JS, Kemp B, Coker J. Depression after spinal cord injury: relation to gender, ethnicity, aging, and socioeconomic indicators. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 2000; 81: 1099–1109.

102. Charlifue S, Gerhart K. Changing psychosocial morbidity in people aging with spinal cord injury. NeuroRehabilitation 2004; 19: 15–23.

103. Geyh S, Ballert C, Sinnott A, Charlifue S, Catz A, D’Andrea Greve JM, et al. Quality of life after spinal cord injury: a comparison across six countries. Spinal Cord 2013; 51: 322–326.

104. Saadat S, Javadi M, Divshali BS, Tavakoli AH, Ghodsi SM, Montazeri A, et al. Health-related quality of life among individuals with long-standing spinal cord injury: a comparative study of veterans and non-veterans. BMC Public Health 2010; 10: 6.

105. Barker RN, Kendall MD, Amsters DI, Pershouse KJ, Haines TP, Kuipers P. The relationship between quality of life and disability across the lifespan for people with spinal cord injury. Spinal Cord 2009; 47: 149–155.

106. Middleton J, Tran Y, Craig A. Relationship between quality of life and self-efficacy in persons with spinal cord injuries. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 2007; 88: 1643–1648.

107. Pershouse KJ, Barker RN, Kendall MB, Buettner PG, Kuipers P, Schuurs SB, et al. Investigating changes in quality of life and function along the lifespan for people with spinal cord injury. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 2012; 93: 413–419.

108. Stevens SL, Caputo JL, Fuller DK, Morgan DW. Physical activity and quality of life in adults with spinal cord injury. J Spinal Cord Med 2008; 31: 373–378.

109. Groah SL, Charlifue S, Tate D, Jensen MP, Molton IR, Forchheimer M, et al. Spinal cord injury and aging: challenges and recommendations for future research. Am J Phys Med Rehabil 2012; 91: 80–93.

110. Bonanno GA, Kennedy P, Galatzer-Levy IR, Lude P, Elfstrom ML. Trajectories of resilience, depression, and anxiety following spinal cord injury. Rehabil Psychol 2012; 57: 236–247.

111. Volken T. Second-stage non-response in the Swiss health survey: determinants and bias in outcomes. BMC Public Health 2013; 13: 167.

Comments

Do you want to comment on this paper? The comments will show up here and if appropriate the comments will also separately be forwarded to the authors. You need to login/create an account to comment on articles. Click here to login/create an account.